92
Universidade Federal do Rio Grande do Norte Centro de Biociências Programa de Pós-graduação em Psicobiologia Coloração de flores na visão de polinizadores Marilia Fernandes Erickson 2019 Marilia Fernandes Erickson

Marilia Fernandes Erickson...Agradeço às minhas irmãs Biny e Becky que, embora não fizeram nenhum bolo de cenoura para mim, estavam sempre com o Ifoods na mão. Aos meus pais,

  • Upload
    others

  • View
    1

  • Download
    0

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

Page 1: Marilia Fernandes Erickson...Agradeço às minhas irmãs Biny e Becky que, embora não fizeram nenhum bolo de cenoura para mim, estavam sempre com o Ifoods na mão. Aos meus pais,

Universidade Federal do Rio Grande do Norte

Centro de Biociências

Programa de Pós-graduação em Psicobiologia

Coloração de flores na visão de polinizadores

Marilia Fernandes Erickson

2019

Marilia Fernandes Erickson

Page 2: Marilia Fernandes Erickson...Agradeço às minhas irmãs Biny e Becky que, embora não fizeram nenhum bolo de cenoura para mim, estavam sempre com o Ifoods na mão. Aos meus pais,

Coloração de flores na visão de polinizadores

Essa dissertação foi desenvolvida no Laboratório de

Ecologia Sensorial do Departamento de Fisiologia e

Comportamento da Universidade Federal do Rio

Grande do Norte, sob orientação do Prof. Dr. Daniel

Marques de Almeida Pessoa e co-orientação do

Professor Carlos Roberto Sorensen Dutra da

Fonseca

Natal

2019

Page 3: Marilia Fernandes Erickson...Agradeço às minhas irmãs Biny e Becky que, embora não fizeram nenhum bolo de cenoura para mim, estavam sempre com o Ifoods na mão. Aos meus pais,

Universidade Federal do Rio Grande do Norte - UFRN

Sistema de Bibliotecas - SISBI

Catalogação de Publicação na Fonte. UFRN - Biblioteca Setorial Prof. Leopoldo Nelson - ­Centro de Biociências - CB

Erickson, Marília Fernandes.

Coloração de flores na visão de polinizadores / Marília Fernandes Erickson. - Natal, 2019.

92 f.: il.

Dissertação (Mestrado) - Universidade Federal do Rio Grande do

Norte. Centro de Biociências. Programa de Pós-graduação em Psicobiologia.

Orientador: Prof. Dr. Daniel Marques de Almeida Pessoa.

1. Angiospermas - Dissertação. 2. Visão de cores -

Dissertação. 3. Modelagem visual - Dissertação. 4. Visitantes

florais - Dissertação. 5. Polinização - Dissertação. I. Pessoa, Daniel Marques de Almeida. II. Universidade Federal do Rio

Grande do Norte. III. Título.

RN/UF/BSE-CB CDU 582.5/.9

Elaborado por KATIA REJANE DA SILVA - CRB-15/351

Page 4: Marilia Fernandes Erickson...Agradeço às minhas irmãs Biny e Becky que, embora não fizeram nenhum bolo de cenoura para mim, estavam sempre com o Ifoods na mão. Aos meus pais,

Título

Coloração de flores na visão de polinizadores

Banca examinadora

Prof. Dr. Felipe Gawryszewski

Universidade de Brasília

________________________________________________________

Prof. Dr. Leonardo M. Versieux

Universidade Federal do Rio Grande do Norte

________________________________________________________

Prof. Dr. Daniel Marques de Almeida Pessoa (orientador)

Universidade Federal do Rio Grande do Norte

________________________________________________________

Natal, 27 de maio de 2019.

Page 5: Marilia Fernandes Erickson...Agradeço às minhas irmãs Biny e Becky que, embora não fizeram nenhum bolo de cenoura para mim, estavam sempre com o Ifoods na mão. Aos meus pais,

i

“Que terra mais pachorrenta!” comentou a

Rainha. “Pois aqui, como vê, você tem de

correr o mais que pode para continuar no

mesmo lugar. Se quiser ir a alguma outra

parte, tem de correr no mínimo duas vezes

mais rápido!”

Alice no País das Maravilhas, Lewis Carroll.

Page 6: Marilia Fernandes Erickson...Agradeço às minhas irmãs Biny e Becky que, embora não fizeram nenhum bolo de cenoura para mim, estavam sempre com o Ifoods na mão. Aos meus pais,

ii

Dedico esta dissertação a Ricardo Andreazze, sua

luz continua a me guiar pelos momentos mais

escuros.

Page 7: Marilia Fernandes Erickson...Agradeço às minhas irmãs Biny e Becky que, embora não fizeram nenhum bolo de cenoura para mim, estavam sempre com o Ifoods na mão. Aos meus pais,

iii

AGRADECIMENTOS

Agradeço às minhas irmãs Biny e Becky que, embora não fizeram nenhum bolo de cenoura para

mim, estavam sempre com o Ifoods na mão.

Aos meus pais, Sandra e Glenn, para os quais poderia escrever uma dissertação inteira dizendo

tudo que fizeram por mim, mas, por hora, vou apenas dizer que, junto com o CNPQ, financiaram

minha pesquisa.

Ao Diogo, que foi “troxa” o suficiente para me emprestar suas pernas, cabeça, e tempo para me

ajudar com esse projeto, seja emocionalmente, conceitualmente ou fisicamente. Tenho absoluta

certeza que não teria conseguido sem sua ajuda em todos esses aspectos.

Ao Daniel pela orientação, confiança e incentivo desde a graduação.

Aos meus colegas de laboratório, cujo apoio foi indispensável. A Mariana por todas as semanas

juntas em Assu, cheias de intrigas, pokemons e brigadeiro. A Sofia, Holda e Raiane, as flores

mais preciosas que encontrei nesse mestrado. A Joaquim, Vinicius, Bia, Thiago, Geovan, Felipe,

Elder, André, Larissa, Amanda e Kleytone. Obrigada por cada um carregar um pedacinho do

trabalho do outro deixando tudo mais leve.

À UFRN, Centro de Biociências, Departamento de Fisiologia e Comportamento, e todos os seus

funcionários.

Ao ICMbio pela manutenção das reservas e permissão de coleta, e alojamento durante a

pesquisa.

Ao Mauro, “Irmão”, Chiquinho, Luiz, e todo pessoal da FLONA de ASSU e da REBIO

Guaribas.

Ao Anderson, Alan e todo o herbário do Parque das Dunas e UFRN, por me cederem material,

tempo e apoio.

Ao Felipe, Letícia, Andréia e Ana Cecília, amigos maravilhosos que o mestrado me presenteou.

Especialmente a “Terceiro” por me ceder a cadeira no ônibus e ótimos papos de calçada.

À Rayane, Eva e Rizia por me fazerem companhia nas horas inacabáveis de laboratório.

À minha melhor amiga Camila, e todos os meus amigos que tentaram me distrair da vida

acadêmica (ou não), das mais diversas formas, Laila, Juh, Thaís, Ana Luíza, Pocati, Pablo,

Daniel, Allan, Gabi, Sarah, Rodolfo e Valéria. À Larissa por ser a melhor motorista anti-

bolsonaro e à Aninha por todas as dicas maravilhosas.

Ao Fábio, Gabriel, David, Rafael e Guilherme amigos que nem o mestrado conseguiram separar.

Ao CNPq pela concessão da bolsa.

O presente trabalho foi realizado com apoio da Coordenação de Aperfeiçoamento de Pessoal de

Nível Superior - Brasil (CAPES) - Código de Financiamento 001

Page 8: Marilia Fernandes Erickson...Agradeço às minhas irmãs Biny e Becky que, embora não fizeram nenhum bolo de cenoura para mim, estavam sempre com o Ifoods na mão. Aos meus pais,

iv

Resumo

A coloração das flores é tão intrigante quanto os fatores ecológicos e ambientas por trás delas.

Desde os primórdios dos estudos de biologia floral, o porquê da coloração das flores vem sendo

questionado. Muitos autores têm atribuído a coloração floral à seleção sexual e pressão exercida

por polinizadores. Isso pode ser bem exemplificado pela ideia de síndromes de polinização;

espécies de flores com certas características semelhantes, como a cor, são visitadas por grupos

similares de polinizadores. Porém, colorações diversas raramente são explicadas por um único

fator. Nesse estudo, procuramos entender quais são os fatores ambientais, ecológicos e fisiológicos

responsáveis pela coloração das flores, com ênfase em testar se espécies previstas por síndromes

de polinização realmente são conspícuas para seus polinizadores. Utilizamos como modelo Apis

mellifera (abelha), Drosophila melanogaster (mosca), Heliconius erato (borboleta) e Sephanoides

sephanoides (beija-flor) para entender como polinizadores diferentes enxergam flores. As flores

foram mais conspícuas para polinizadores tetracromatas (mosca, borboleta fêmea, e beija-flor) e

menos conspícuas para polinizadores tricromatas (borboleta macho, abelha). Dessa maneira, flores

não foram mais conspícuas para seus polinizadores, e cores atribuídas as síndromes de polinização

não possuem bases empíricas. Provavelmente diferentes fatores interagem para moldar a coloração

das flores ao longo do tempo e síndromes de polinização são apenas um recorte de uma figura mais

complexa.

Palavras chave: Angiospermas, visão de cores, modelagem visual, visitantes florais, polinização.

Page 9: Marilia Fernandes Erickson...Agradeço às minhas irmãs Biny e Becky que, embora não fizeram nenhum bolo de cenoura para mim, estavam sempre com o Ifoods na mão. Aos meus pais,

v

Abstract

Flower coloration is as intriguing as the ecological and environmental factors behind it. Since the

beginning of studies in floral biology, the question of the reasons behind floral coloration has been

asked. Many authors have attributed flower colors to sexual selection and pollinator pressure. This

is well exemplified by the idea of pollination syndromes: flowers with certain similar

characteristics, such as color, are visited by similar groups of pollinators. Such a diverse array of

coloration, however, is hardly ever explained by one factor alone. In this study, we aimed at

understanding which environmental, ecological and physiological pressures are behind flower

coloration, emphasizing, in testing, if flowers predicted by pollination syndromes are in fact

conspicuous to their pollinators. We used Apis mellifera (honeybee), Drosophila melanogaster

(housefly), Heliconius erato (butterfly) and Sephanoides sephanoides (hummingbird) as models

to study how different pollinators see flowers. Flowers were more conspicuous to tetrachromat

(housefly, female butterfly and hummingbird) than to trichromat (honeybee and male butterfly)

pollinators. Therefore, flowers were not more conspicuous for their respective pollinators, and

colors attributed by pollination syndromes do are not supported by empirical data. Probably

different factors have shaped the coloration of flowers across time, and pollination syndromes are

a piece of the whole picture.

Keywords: Angiosperms, color vision, flower color, floral visitors, pollination.

Page 10: Marilia Fernandes Erickson...Agradeço às minhas irmãs Biny e Becky que, embora não fizeram nenhum bolo de cenoura para mim, estavam sempre com o Ifoods na mão. Aos meus pais,

vi

Lista de Figuras

Estudo 1- Painting the roses red: Temporal-spatial patterns and the evolution of flower

coloration

Figura 1. Number of articles in web of science regarding flower coloration in the last

decades…………………….......................................................................................................p. 20

Figura 2. Diagram exemplifying how flower color is affected by different factors discussed in this

review.........................................................................................................................................p. 40

Estudo 2- Pollination syndromes do not predict conspicuousness by different floral visitors

Figura 1. Boxplot showing distribution of chromatic contrast values between flowers and

background elements, comparison is within pollinators, and flowers are grouped by

syndrome...................................................................................................................................p. 64

Figura 2. Boxplot showing distribution of chromatic contrast values between flowers and

background elements, comparison is within pollinators, and flowers are grouped by

color...........................................................................................................................................p. 66

Figura 3. Boxplot showing distribution of chromatic contrast values between flowers and

background elements, comparison is between pollinators, and flowers are grouped by

syndrome....................................................................................................................................p.67

Figura 4. Boxplot showing distribution of chromatic contrast values between flowers and

background elements, comparison is between pollinators, and flowers are grouped by

color............................................................................................................................................p. 68

Supplementary material 3. Compared reflectance of flowers measured with Ocean Optics RPH-

1 probe holder and our custom-made 3D printed probe holder...................................................p. 85

Page 11: Marilia Fernandes Erickson...Agradeço às minhas irmãs Biny e Becky que, embora não fizeram nenhum bolo de cenoura para mim, estavam sempre com o Ifoods na mão. Aos meus pais,

vii

Supplementary material 4. Illuminant of Caatinga and Restinga used in visual

modeling.....................................................................................................................................p. 85

Page 12: Marilia Fernandes Erickson...Agradeço às minhas irmãs Biny e Becky que, embora não fizeram nenhum bolo de cenoura para mim, estavam sempre com o Ifoods na mão. Aos meus pais,

viii

Lista de Tabelas

Estudo 2- Pollination syndromes do not predict conspicuousness by different floral visitors

Tabela 1. New color categories with description and number of flowers in each group............p. 59

Tabela 2. Parameters used in our visual modeling.....................................................................p. 61

Tabela 3. Number of flowers, of different colors, that are cryptic, barely detectable, and

detectable……………………………........................................................................................p. 63

Supplementary material 1. List of species used, locations found, and which backgrounds were

used for the visual modeling...…………………………………………………………………p. 79

Supplementary material 2. Description of flower color categories, proposed by Wilmer (2011),

and the number of flowers sampled in our study………............................................................p. 84

Page 13: Marilia Fernandes Erickson...Agradeço às minhas irmãs Biny e Becky que, embora não fizeram nenhum bolo de cenoura para mim, estavam sempre com o Ifoods na mão. Aos meus pais,

ix

Sumário

1. Introdução 1.1 Introdução geral..................................................................................................p. 11

1.2 Referencias ........................................................................................................p. 13

2. Objetivos, hipóteses & predições.................................................................................p. 14

2.1 Objetivos............................................................................................................p. 15

2.1.1 Objetivo geral...............................................................................................p. 15

2.1.2 Objetivos específicos....................................................................................p. 15

2.2 Hipóteses e predições.........................................................................................p. 16

3. Artigo 1: Painting the roses red: Temporal-Spatial patterns and the evolution of

flower coloration ..........................................................................................................p. 17

3.1 Abstract..............................................................................................................p. 18

3.2 Introduction........................................................................................................p. 19

3.3 Pigment and flower coloration............................................................................p. 21

3.4 The visual system of pollinators.........................................................................p. 23

3.5 Floral syndromes................................................................................................p. 26

3.6 Phylogenetic constraint......................................................................................p. 29

3.7 Floral age............................................................................................................p. 31

3.8 Seasonal changes in flower color........................................................................p. 33

3.9 Biogeographical changes....................................................................................p. 34

3.10 Sensory drive......................................................................................................p. 37

3.11 Conclusion..........................................................................................................p. 39

3.12 References..........................................................................................................p. 42

4. Artigo 2: Pollination syndromes do not predict conspicuousness by different floral

visitors...........................................................................................................................p. 51

4.1 Abstract..............................................................................................................p. 52

4.2 Introduction........................................................................................................p. 53

4.3 Materials and methods........................................................................................p. 56

4.3.1 Data colection...............................................................................................p. 56

4.3.2 Flower characterization................................................................................p. 57

4.3.3 Visual modeling...........................................................................................p. 59

4.3.4 Analysis........................................................................................................p. 60

4.4 Results................................................................................................................p. 61

4.5 Discussion..........................................................................................................p. 68

4.6 Conclusion..........................................................................................................p. 72

4.7 References..........................................................................................................p. 73

4.8 Supplementary Material.....................................................................................p. 78

5. Conclusão geral.............................................................................................................p. 85

6. Apêndices......................................................................................................................p. 89

6.1 Aprovação do comitê de ética...................................................................................p. 88

Page 14: Marilia Fernandes Erickson...Agradeço às minhas irmãs Biny e Becky que, embora não fizeram nenhum bolo de cenoura para mim, estavam sempre com o Ifoods na mão. Aos meus pais,

10

1.INTRODUÇÃO

Page 15: Marilia Fernandes Erickson...Agradeço às minhas irmãs Biny e Becky que, embora não fizeram nenhum bolo de cenoura para mim, estavam sempre com o Ifoods na mão. Aos meus pais,

11

1.1 Introdução geral

Angiospermas são os organismos autótrofos de maior diversidade que há na Terra. Seu

sucesso reprodutivo é constantemente relacionado com a presença de flores (Armbruster 2014),

seus órgãos reprodutivos. Para angiospermas se reproduzirem, seus gametas precisam ser

transportados por vetores (polinizadores), que podem ser abióticos, como a água e o vento, ou

bióticos, empregando animais. Entre os animais polinizadores, pode-se destacar abelhas,

borboletas, moscas, morcegos e aves. As plantas disponibilizam recursos (que podem ser pólen,

néctar, óleo e fragrâncias) para seus polinizadores que estão forrageando e durante o processo de

forrageio transportam os gametas realizando a polinização (Westerkamp 1996). Para assegurar sua

reprodução, as plantas precisam se comunicar com diferentes animais utilizando, principalmente,

sinais visuais e químicos.

Plantas obtém sua coloração através de pigmentos, que além de servir para atração de

polinizadores, funcionam como defesa química contra herbívoros, proteção contra radiação solar,

entre outras funções. Além disso, plantas estão competindo por polinizadores, o que pode levar a

divergência ou convergência de cores, dependendo do ambiente. Mesmo levando em consideração

somente pressão exercidas por polinizadores, muitos animais apresentam preferências inatas por

cores, e aprendem rapidamente a associar cores com recompensas, o que os influencia diretamente

durante o forrageio.

Os sinais florais mais importantes são os visuais (Waser, Chittka, Prince, Williams &

Ollerton 1996). A coloração é percebida a mais longa distância, em comparação a outros sinais

como cheiro e padrões (Chittka & Menzel 1992). Adicionalmente, a cor é um dos principais

promotores de constância floral (Waser 1986, Chittka & Menzel 1992), isso é, a tendência de

polinizadores restringirem suas visitas a um número limitado de morfotipos de flores (Chittka,

Page 16: Marilia Fernandes Erickson...Agradeço às minhas irmãs Biny e Becky que, embora não fizeram nenhum bolo de cenoura para mim, estavam sempre com o Ifoods na mão. Aos meus pais,

12

Thomson & Waser 1999). A coloração de flores é diversa e intrigante no espectro visível da nossa

espécie. Contudo, adicionalmente, a maioria dos polinizadores ainda conseguem enxergar luz

ultravioleta (UV), revelando padrões nas flores que são imperceptíveis a humanos.

A presente dissertação está dividida em dois artigos. O primeiro artigo é uma revisão

bibliográfica que visa entender quais diferentes fatores podem afetar a coloração das flores.

Começa abordando como diferentes pigmentos e fatores intracelulares causam a coloração das

flores, exemplifica como é o sistema visual de diferentes grupos de polinizadores, e como a pressão

por polinização pode ter moldado a coloração das flores. Então, examina como a coloração das

flores esta sujeita a restrições filogenéticas e como diferentes fatores (e.g. idade, estação do ano e

posição geográfica) podem atuar no direcionamento da evolução da coloração das flores. Por

último, discute como a teoria do direcionamento sensorial pode ajudar a explicar padrões

biogeográficos de coloração de flores. O segundo artigo é um empírico e busca testar se as cores

atribuídas às síndromes de polinização estão relacionadas às capacidades visuais encontradas em

diferentes grupos de polinizadores.

Page 17: Marilia Fernandes Erickson...Agradeço às minhas irmãs Biny e Becky que, embora não fizeram nenhum bolo de cenoura para mim, estavam sempre com o Ifoods na mão. Aos meus pais,

13

1.2 Referências

Armbruster, W. S. (2014). Floral Specialization and Angiosperm Diversity: Phenotypic Divergence,

Fitness Trade-Offs and Realized Pollination Accuracy. Annals of Botany, 6.

https://doi.org/doi:10.1093/aobpla/plu003

Chittka, L., & Menzel, R. (1992). The Evolutionary Adaptation of Flower Colours and the Insect

Pollinators’ Colour Vision. Journal of Comparative Physiology, 171(2), 171–181.

https://doi.org/doi:10.1007/bf00188925

Chittka, L., Shmida, A., Troje, N., & Menzel, R. (1999). Flower Constancy, Insect Psychology, and

Plant Evolution. Naturwissenschaften, 86(8), 361–377. https://doi.org/10.1007/s001140050636

Waser, N. M. (1986). Flower Constancy: Definition, Cause, and Measurement. The American

Naturalist, 127(5), 593–603. https://doi.org/DOI: 10.1086/284507

Waser, N. M., Chittka, L., Prince, M. V., Williams, N. M., & Ollerton, J. (1996). Generalization in

Pollination Systems, and Why it Matters. Ecology, 77(4), 1043–1060. https://doi.org/DOI:

10.2307/2265575

Westerkamp, C. (996). Pollen in Bee‐Flower Relations Some Considerations on Melittophily*.

Botanica Acta, 109, 325–332. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1438-8677.1996.tb00580.x

Page 18: Marilia Fernandes Erickson...Agradeço às minhas irmãs Biny e Becky que, embora não fizeram nenhum bolo de cenoura para mim, estavam sempre com o Ifoods na mão. Aos meus pais,

14

2. OBJETIVOS, HIPÓTESES E PREDIÇÕES

__________________________________________________________

Page 19: Marilia Fernandes Erickson...Agradeço às minhas irmãs Biny e Becky que, embora não fizeram nenhum bolo de cenoura para mim, estavam sempre com o Ifoods na mão. Aos meus pais,

15

2.1. Objetivos

2.1.1.Objetivo geral

Investigar o papel da visão de polinizadores na evolução da coloração floral.

2.1.2.Objetivos específicos

I- Revisar a literatura sobre fatores que influenciam na evolução da coloração das

flores.

II- Examinar o papel da visão de cores de diferentes visitantes florais no

estabelecimento de síndromes de polinização.

Page 20: Marilia Fernandes Erickson...Agradeço às minhas irmãs Biny e Becky que, embora não fizeram nenhum bolo de cenoura para mim, estavam sempre com o Ifoods na mão. Aos meus pais,

16

2.2. Hipóteses e predições

I. Hipótese 1: Flores apresentam maior contraste de cor entre a pétala e seu background para

seu respectivo polinizador

a. Predição 1: Flores melitofilas (polinizadas por abelhas) terão maior contraste de cor, com

relação à folhagem, quando visualizadas por abelhas; flores psicofilas (polinizadas por

borboletas) terão contraste maior para borboletas, flores miofilas (polinizadas por moscas)

terão contraste maior para moscas e flores ornitófilas (polinizadas por aves) terão contraste

maior para aves.

b. Predição 2: Flores azuis, que seriam preferencialmente utilizadas por abelhas, terão maior

contraste de cor, quando comparadas à vegetação, quando visualizadas por abelhas, em

comparação à visão de outro polinizadores; flores vermelhas terão contraste maior para

aves e borboletas; flores brancas terão contraste maior para moscas e abelhas; flores verdes

terão contraste maior para moscas; flores amarelas terão contraste maior para abelhas,

moscas e borboletas (Wilmer 2011).

II. Hipótese 2: Polinizadores terão uma melhor detecção de cores daquelas flores que se

encaixam em suas síndromes de polinização.

a. Predição 3: Abelhas irão enxergar melhor flores melitofilas, quando comparadas a flores

das demais síndromes florais; aves irão enxergar melhor ornitófilas, moscas irão enxergar

melhor flores psicofilas, e borboletas irão enxergar melhor flores psicofilas.

b. Predição 4: Abelhas irão enxergar melhor flores azuis, amarelas e brancas; aves irão

enxergar melhor flores vermelhas; moscas irão enxergar melhor flores brancas, verdes e

amarelas; borboletas irão enxergar melhor flores amarelas e vermelhas.

Page 21: Marilia Fernandes Erickson...Agradeço às minhas irmãs Biny e Becky que, embora não fizeram nenhum bolo de cenoura para mim, estavam sempre com o Ifoods na mão. Aos meus pais,

17

3. ARTIGO 1

Painting the Roses Red: Temporal-spatial Patterns and the Evolution of

Flower Coloration

Page 22: Marilia Fernandes Erickson...Agradeço às minhas irmãs Biny e Becky que, embora não fizeram nenhum bolo de cenoura para mim, estavam sempre com o Ifoods na mão. Aos meus pais,

18

Painting the Roses Red: Temporal-spatial Patterns and the Evolution of Flower Coloration

Marilia Erickson1 and Daniel M. A. Pessoa

Laboratory of Sensory Ecology, Department of Physiology and Behavior, Universidade Federal do Rio Grande

do Norte. Natal – RN, Brazil. CEP 59078-970

1- MSc student; E-mail: [email protected]

3.1 Abstract

The diversity of flower color has always been puzzling. Though flower coloration has been

extensively studied, many unanswered questions remain. Studies on the coloration of flowers focus

extensively on pollination. Flower coloration, however, has multiple functions, such as protecting

against herbivory and other harmful visitors, and preventing ultraviolet damage. Here we review

different factors affecting coloration in flowers by using a visual communication perspective, since

recent studies have shown many similarities between strategies of animal and plant coloration,

such as aposematism, camouflage, mimicry and private communication channels. We begin by

looking at how plants produce pigment and how various receivers process coloration. Then we

explore the ultimate (e.g. pollinator pressures and phylogenetic restraints) and proximate (e.g.

effects of ontogeny on coloration, a bewildering phenomenon known as flower color change)

causes of flower coloration, as well as the temporal and spatial patterns in flower communities.

Finally, we look at how sensory drive could have framed the evolution of flower color. In short,

we aim to contribute to ongoing research by underlining the main current topics in flower

coloration studies, indicating perspectives for future studies of floral color.

Keywords: Pigments, color vision, pollination ecology, flower ontogeny

Page 23: Marilia Fernandes Erickson...Agradeço às minhas irmãs Biny e Becky que, embora não fizeram nenhum bolo de cenoura para mim, estavam sempre com o Ifoods na mão. Aos meus pais,

19

3.2 Introduction

There are an estimated 308,000 plant species in the world that depend on animals for

pollination (Ollerton, Winfree & Tarrant 2011) and hence need to overcome communication

barriers between different species in order to reproduce. Plants are also subject to herbivory, nectar

robbing and other antagonistic behavior from animals who can often explore the same sensory

modalities of pollinators. These pressures can lead flowers to diverge or converge in color with

flowers in their community. In general, having a distinct coloration from neighbors helps with

flower constancy and is a favorable strategy (Waser 1986, Chittka, Thomson & Waser 1999,

Schaefer, Schaefer & Levey 2004). Yet this is not always the case, insofar as some mimetic plants

depend on similar colors for pollination (Peter & Johnson 2008). Pollinator preference may also

play a role in determining coloration (Dyer et al. 2012). Additionally, protection against herbivory

(Irwin, Strauss, Storz, Emerson & Guibert 2003, Johnson, Berhow & Dowd 2008), UV light

(Kootstra 1994; Mori et al. 2005), seasonal change (Stace & Fripp 1977, Hensel & Sargent 2012)

and habitat (Gumbert, Kunze & Chittka 1999; Arnold, Savolainen & Chittka 2009; Shrestha, Dyer,

Bhattarai & Burd 2014) can all play a selective role in coloration.

Nonetheless, plants have been widely overlooked by researchers as communicating organisms.

Although the science of floral coloration is a growing topic, there is a strong bias towards the

publication of pollination studies (13,400 records in web of science – from 1970 to 2019), leaving

other research topics largely underexplored, such as: flower color change (80 records in Web of

Science Database – from 1970 to 2019), camouflage (30 – from 1970 to 2019), aposematism (17

– from 1970 to 2019), private communication channels (9 – from 1970 to 2019) and sensory drive

(7 – from 1970 to 2019) (Fig 1). The good news are that plant signaling have growingly received

more attention in the past years, dealing with concepts that have only been thoroughly researched

Page 24: Marilia Fernandes Erickson...Agradeço às minhas irmãs Biny e Becky que, embora não fizeram nenhum bolo de cenoura para mim, estavam sempre com o Ifoods na mão. Aos meus pais,

20

in animals, such as mimetism (Lunau & Wester 2017), aposematism (Lev-Yadun & Gould 2007,

Lev-Yadun, Ne'eman & Keasar 2017), camouflage (Shuttleworth & Johnson 2009; Niu, Sun &

Stevens 2018), signal honesty (Makino & Ohashi 2017), private communication channels

(Shuttleworth & Johnson 2009; Lunau, Papiorek, Eltz & Sazima 2011) and sensory drive

(Schaefer, Schaefer & Levey 2004).

0

1000

2000

3000

4000

5000

6000

7000

8000

1970-1979 1980-1989 1990-1999 2000-2009 2010-2019

Nu

mb

er o

f re

cord

s in

Web

of

Scie

nce

Dat

abas

e

time period

flower OR floral colo* AND pollination

flower OR floral colo* AND pigment

flower OR floral colo* AND herbivory

flower OR floral colo* AND mimetism or deception or mimicry

flower OR floral colo* AND phylogenetic constraint

flo* colo* change

flower OR floral colo* AND camuflage or criptic or crypsis

flower OR floral colo* AND aposematism OR "warning colo*"

flower OR floral colo* AND "private niche" or "private channel"

flower OR floral colo* AND sensory drive

Page 25: Marilia Fernandes Erickson...Agradeço às minhas irmãs Biny e Becky que, embora não fizeram nenhum bolo de cenoura para mim, estavam sempre com o Ifoods na mão. Aos meus pais,

21

Figure 1. Number of articles in web of science regarding flower coloration on the last decades.

The terms were inputted on the web of science main collection on June of 2019. Keywords used

are presented in the figure labeled by different colors.

In this paper, we aim to review different factors that affect coloration in flowers (Fig 2). We

start by looking at how plants produce color and how different receivers process visual

information. Then we explore the ultimate and proximate causes of flower coloration, which

include pollinators pressure, flower age, season and habitat as well as phylogeny. Finally, we

discuss how sensory drive could explain convergent evolution in flower communities. We end this

paper with some prospects of future studies regarding flower coloration.

3.3 Pigment and flower coloration

Pigments are molecules that absorb some wavelengths of light and reflect others. The reflected

wavelengths give color to objects; and so, pigment determines the reflectance of flowers (Chittka,

Shmida, Troje & Menzel 1994). There are three major group of plant pigments: flavonoids,

carotenoids and betalains. Their core structures differ in light absorption properties and can also

be attached to other chemical groups to form more variable flower colorations (Wilmer 2011).

Different concentration of pigment can affect most of the parameters used for studying color

vision, including dominant wavelength, spectral purity, green contrast and color contrast

(Papiorek, Rohde & Lunau 2013).

The diversity of flower color is often attributed to pollination pressure and sexual selection

(Schiestl & Johnson 2013). Selection for pigment coloration, however, goes beyond pollinator

choice. Some pigments are also associated with chemical defenses against herbivory, this being

one of the hypotheses as to why there are different color morphs in the same species. In the wild

radish, Raphanus sativus L., pollinators prefer white and yellow morphs, which have a lower

Page 26: Marilia Fernandes Erickson...Agradeço às minhas irmãs Biny e Becky que, embora não fizeram nenhum bolo de cenoura para mim, estavam sempre com o Ifoods na mão. Aos meus pais,

22

concentration of anthocyanins, in comparison to bronze and pink color morphs that have a high

concentration of pigment (Stanton 1987). The color morphs with lower anthocyanin concentration,

however, are less resistant to herbivory, which can provide a selective pressure to maintain high

pigment morphs (Irwin, Strauss, Storz, Emerson & Guibert 2003). In star-patterned petunia,

Petunia hybrida Vilm., flowers are multicolored, having a white star pattern at the middle of the

corolla that can have multiple colors surrounding it. The colored part has a higher concentration

of anthocyanins and was found to slow the development of lepidopteran larvae (Johnson, Berhow

& Dowd 2008). Thus, it is possible that herbivores avoid plants colored by anthocyanins because

they indicate presence of defensive compounds (Schaefer & Rolshausen 2006), a tendency that,

perhaps, should also be regarded as aposematism (Lev-Yadun & Gould 2007).

Pigments can also block UV radiation and prevent DNA damage (Kootstra 1994; Mori et al.

2005). Accumulation of protective anthocyanins caused by UV radiation produce red to purple

colors in exposed tissue (Burger & Edwards 1996), as seems to be the case in Delachampia and

Acer, in which flower color seems to be associated with the presence of anthocyanin in vegetative

tissue (Armbruster 2002). Plant pigments have also been associated with other functions such as

drought resistance, temperature resistance, heavy metal resistance, and antioxidative capabilities

(Chalker-Scott 1999, Gould 2004, Pourcel, Routaboul, Cheynier, Lepiniec & Debeaujon 2007).

Presence of pigment alone, however, does not determine flower color. Cellular pH and cellular

architecture may have a major role in determining flower coloration (Grotewold 2006). Varieties

of Antirrhinum majus L. are perceived differently by their pollinators when having equal pigment

concentration but differing cell shape (Glover & Martin 1998). Flowers can also reflect iridescent

light due to structural mechanisms (Whitney et al 2009, Glover & Whitney 2010). Likewise, purple

and blue flower variants of Ipomoea nil (L.) Roth do not differ in pigment concentration, but in

Page 27: Marilia Fernandes Erickson...Agradeço às minhas irmãs Biny e Becky que, embora não fizeram nenhum bolo de cenoura para mim, estavam sempre com o Ifoods na mão. Aos meus pais,

23

sap pH (Fukada-Tanaka, Inagaki, Yamaguchi, Saito & Iida 2000). Hence, biochemistry of flower

coloration can be either determined by pigments, pH and cellular structure and influenced by

external factors.

3.4 The visual system of pollinators

Among the many functions of pigments, signaling is the most important (Schiestl & Johnson

2013). Through color, flowers attract or repel visitors (Fig 2). Plants have little plasticity in

signaling capacities, signals being seen by mutualist and antagonist (animals that visit plants or

flowers and have harmful interactions with them) alike (Schaefer, Schaefer & Levey 2004). How

these signals are interpreted, however, depends on the receiver’s sensory capacity. Communication

through color requires animals to have a visual system that detects and interprets flower color. The

main flower visitors are insects, mostly because of their function as pollinators, but also because

they are quite vicious herbivores, florivores, nectar-robbers, pollen thieves, sapsuckers, and

parasites. Hence, animals with very similar visual systems can be either beneficial or harmful to

the same plant. We will, however, focus this review on pollinator’s visual ecology, inasmuch as

pollinator choice is seen as the primary function of flower color (Schiestl & Johnson 2013), and

since very few studies have considered eavesdroppers as a selective pressure for color (Cuthill et

al 2017).

Insects have varied visual systems. Yet most insects seem to be trichromats with preserved

photoreceptors that detect light in the UV, blue and green part of the electromagnetic spectrum

(Briscoe & Chittka 2001). Hymenoptera (e.g. ants, bees and wasps) have had their visual pigments

extensively studied (Peitish et al. 1992) and follow, with few exceptions, the UV-blue-green

receptor pattern found in most other insects. Bees are very visually oriented and can use achromatic

and chromatic color vision for the detection of flowers (Giufa, Vorobyev, Kevan & Menzel 1996).

Page 28: Marilia Fernandes Erickson...Agradeço às minhas irmãs Biny e Becky que, embora não fizeram nenhum bolo de cenoura para mim, estavam sempre com o Ifoods na mão. Aos meus pais,

24

They are quick to associate flower color and reward and can maintain flower constancy, that is,

the habit of a flower visitor to effectively restrict their visits to a few flower species or morphs

(Chittka, Thomson & Waser 1999). Flower constancy is very important for plants, insofar as it

ensures the pollen will go to another individual of the same species without misplacement of pollen

(Muchhala & Thomson 2012) or development of infertile hybrids (Heinrich 1975). Leaves, stones

and other general background components are achromatic to bees (Chittka, Shmida, Troje &

Menzel 1994), making bees ideal for detecting flowers (Chittka 1997).

Hoverflies usually have four photoreceptor varieties (Lunau 2014). Yet their color vision has

been interpreted as relatively poor, since they would be able to distinguish only four color

categories (Troje 1993), with scents being more important than colors (Roy & Rasugo 1997).

However, recently, objections to this model have appeared (White, Dalrymple, Herberstein &

Kemp 2017), as Drosophila melanogaster (housefly) has been able to distinguish flowers within

the same category (Brembs & de Ibarra 2006). The visual ecology of flies is still poorly understood

(Lunau 2014).

The most common change in receptors within insects was the addition of a red receptor, which

has happened independently many times within Lepidoptera (Briscoe & Chittka 2001). Butterflies

may have from as few as three to as many as fifteen kinds of photoreceptors, though most

butterflies have six different spectral sensitivities (Arikawa 2017). It is often assumed that the

number of classes of photoreceptors determines the dimension of color vision, but this is not

always the case (Cuthill et al 2017). Butterflies, despite usually having six different kinds of

photoreceptors, have tetrachromatic vision using the UV, blue, green and red receptors (Arikawa

2017). They are capable of seeing the entire color spectrum, leading to great color discrimination,

usually associated with sexual selection and foraging (Kelber 2016). Moths, like bees, have UV,

Page 29: Marilia Fernandes Erickson...Agradeço às minhas irmãs Biny e Becky que, embora não fizeram nenhum bolo de cenoura para mim, estavam sempre com o Ifoods na mão. Aos meus pais,

25

blue and green receptors that may be used for color vision even at night (Kelber, Balkenius &

Wattant 2003).

Some vertebrates can also play a key role in pollination and visitation of flowers. For instance,

bats are common vertebrate pollinators for nocturnal flowers. Although bats are known for

echolocation, they also have dichromatic color vision, in the UV and green range (Müller et al.

2009). Hummingbirds are mainly diurnal visitors and have four receptor types: UV, blue, green

and red (Herrera et al 2008), so are able to detect the entire color spectrum, like butterflies. It is a

misconception that birds prefer red flowers, as studies have shown birds not to have innate color

preferences (Lunau, Papiorek, Eltz & Sazima 2011). Birds rely little on smell, and largely use

visual cues for detection of flowers, which makes them excellent models to study the evolution of

floral color due to pollinator preference.

Although flowers are visited by animals with different visual systems, Chittka & Menzel

(1992) and Dyer et al. (2012) argue that hymenoptera are the main drivers for the evolution of

flower coloration. Luckily, bees have one of the most studied color vision systems, second only to

primates (Wilmer 2011). Different methods can be used to study color vision such as visual

modeling (Stevens, Stoddard & Higham 2009, Renoult, Kelber & Schaefer 2017, Gawryszewski

2018, Olsson, Lind, & Kelber, 2018), comparing the raw reflectance of flowers (Chittka, Shmida,

Troje & Menzel 1994; Arnold, Comber & Chittka 2009) and using behavioral experiments Dyer

(2012). Studies that investigate the role of more than one visual system are still relatively scarce

(Schaefer, Schaefer & Levey 2004) and should be encouraged, especially considering those

investigating antagonistic interactions.

Page 30: Marilia Fernandes Erickson...Agradeço às minhas irmãs Biny e Becky que, embora não fizeram nenhum bolo de cenoura para mim, estavam sempre com o Ifoods na mão. Aos meus pais,

26

3.5 Floral syndromes

Different groups of floral visitors may have differential pollinator efficiency, which can be

measured by seed production. In Calathea sp., Hesperiidae butterflies account for 21% of visits

but for less than 1% of seed set. Bombus medius (bumblebee) and Rhathymus sp (bee), however,

only had 5% of visits but were responsible for 22% of seed set. The reproductive success of a

plant, therefore, is dependent on the kind of visitor it attracts (Schemske & Horvitz 1984). For a

long time, flowers were grouped by their morphological characteristics in a pollination syndrome

according to which pollinator it was supposedly meant to attract. The classical pollination

syndromes are anemophily (wind), hydrophily (water), cantharophily (beetles), myophily (flies),

sapromyophily (carrion and dung flies), psychophily (butterflies), phalaenophily (moths),

melittophily (bees), ornitophily (birds) and chiropterophily (bats). Flower syndromes are based on

the idea that some characteristics are overrepresented in flowers pollinated by certain agent. So, as

a result of pollinator pressure, flowers will lead to convergent evolution of flower traits. Thus, for

instance, a flower that has anthesis during the day, red or orange coloration, no scent or nectar

guides, conceals nectar in high volume and low sugar concentration, and has low amounts of pollen

and radial or bilateral symmetry, with short to medium corolla tube length, will likely be pollinated

by birds (Wilmer 2011).

Trait convergence is often used as a predictive characteristic for a plant’s pollinator. Floral

characteristics of color, flower type, corolla width, and presence of nectar, correctly identified 86%

of bird pollinated species, 78% of fly pollinated species and 69% of bee pollinated species in

Australian heath Styphelioideae (Johnson 2013). In snapdragons, Antirrhineae, flower

morphology, which included flower color and 7 other characteristics, had an overall positive

Page 31: Marilia Fernandes Erickson...Agradeço às minhas irmãs Biny e Becky que, embora não fizeram nenhum bolo de cenoura para mim, estavam sempre com o Ifoods na mão. Aos meus pais,

27

predictive value (PPV) of 65.95% for pollinators and flower visitors (Guzmán, Gómez & Vargas

2017).

Even though studies have shown some potential for the use of floral syndromes in the study of

the evolution of flower traits, recently the idea of pollinator syndromes has fallen under lots of

criticism. First, there are far more distinct flower morphologies than types of pollinators (Heinrich

1975). Second, most animal floral visitors visit more than one plant species, and most plant species

receive visits from more than one animal group, both animals and plants alike being opportunistic

about pollination (Waser, Chittka, Prince, Williams & Ollerton 1996). Third, pollinators are

foraging for food while plants use them as a vector for gamete transportation and hence,

reproduction, what some authors better describe as a mutual exploitation system rather than a

mutualistic relationship (Westerkamp 1996). Fourth, the tendency in nature seems to be

generalization of plant-pollinator systems (Waser, Chittka, Prince, Williams & Ollerton 1996).

And fifth, floral constancy favors diversity of signal (Waser 1986, Schaefer, Schaefer & Levey

2004), which does not support to the convergent notion of pollination syndromes (Fig 2).

Indeed, floral color does not always seem to be associated with pollinator syndrome. Kuniyasu

et al (1998) associated flowers of a lowland dipterocarp forest in Sarawak (Malaysia) with

pollination syndromes and found that pollination syndromes do relate with certain flower

characteristics, such as reward, shape and flowering time, but not with color. Opomopsis aggregate

(Pursh) V. E. Grant is morphologically a bird-pollinated flower (red with tubular corolla tube),

and indeed most its visits are from hummingbirds. Bumblebees, however, outperform

hummingbirds in cross pollen deposition by three times, and induce four times more seed

production in O. aggregata (Mayfield, Waser & Prince 2001). In the genus Ichoroma, though

species with high nectar reward and large floral display were more likely to be pollinized by

Page 32: Marilia Fernandes Erickson...Agradeço às minhas irmãs Biny e Becky que, embora não fizeram nenhum bolo de cenoura para mim, estavam sempre com o Ifoods na mão. Aos meus pais,

28

hummingbirds, corolla length and flower color were not found to be associated with pollinator

groups (Smith, Ané, Baum 2008).

A different view from the traditional pollination syndromes is that visitors cluster in functional

groups of pollinators (e.g. long-tongued flies). Functional groups will exercise similar evolutionary

pressures causing correlation among floral traits (Fenster, Armbruster, Wilson, Dudash &

Thomson 2004). Using the idea of functional groups, Fenster et al. (2004) reanalyzed the same

data as used by Waser, Chittka, Prince, Williams, & Ollerton (1996) and found that 75% of species

exhibited specialization in terms of functional groups. Functional groups are not, however, a clear-

cut solution to the criticism of pollinator syndromes. Ollerton et al. (2009) surveyed flowers from

three different continents and tried to test if floral syndromes, using functional groups, could

predict the most common pollinator of different flowers. They found that even though floral

syndrome characteristics formed a cluster in a phenotypical space, they were mostly unoccupied.

The minority of species fell into clusters formed by the pollination syndrome’s characteristics, as

most species were located between clusters. These results further the understanding that real

species do not comply to the long-established notion of pollination syndromes.

Another explanation for convergence of certain flower traits is that plants can evolve

characteristics to exclude certain visitors (Heinrich 1975). That is well within the idea of private

communication channels, that is, a communication system that involves a signal to which an

eavesdropper is insensitive (Stevens 2013). That might explain the ornithophily syndrome, which

states bird flowers are usually red. Bees are generally insensitive to longer wavelengths (Peitsch

et al. 1992), which serves to generate a private communication channel for hummingbirds (Lunau,

Papiorek, Eltz & Sazima 2011), excluding bee visitors that can be nectar robbers in hummingbird

pollinated species (Irwin & Brody 2000). Other ways to exclude unwanted visits, is by camouflage.

Page 33: Marilia Fernandes Erickson...Agradeço às minhas irmãs Biny e Becky que, embora não fizeram nenhum bolo de cenoura para mim, estavam sempre com o Ifoods na mão. Aos meus pais,

29

Flowers of Eucomis autumnalis (Mill.) Chitt. and Eucomis comosa (Houtt.) H. R. Wehrh. are

visually cryptic by having similar color to leaves, attracting pollinators solely by smell

(Shuttleworth & Johnson 2009).

Accordingly, it appears that even though pollination syndromes were not developed to be

diagnostic (Wilmer 2011), the predictive power of pollination syndromes is limited and frequently

overstated. Interestingly, pollinators seem to prefer a certain flower color, but flower color does

not determine pollinator assemblage, supporting the notion that generalization seems to be a more

frequent flower strategy (Reverté, Retana, Gómez & Bosch 2016). In Erysimum, lilac flowers were

related to a pollinator niche comprised of large long-tonged bees, but it seems that the development

of lilac flowers pre-dates pollinator preference and is probably related to other environmental

factors which eventually led to bee pollination (Gómez, Perfectti & Lorite 2015). The evidence for

the hypothesis that plants develop certain colors to attract certain animals is controversial, as

pollinator preference may lead to divergence of floral color rather than convergence (Schaefer,

Schaefer & Levey 2004, Arnold, Comber & Chittka 2009). Furthermore, the visual system of

pollinator does not seem to be adapted to specific color preferences, as suggested by pollination

syndromes (M. Erickson, D. J. A. Silva & D. M. A. Pessoa, in preparation). Overall, pollinators

still provide a strong selective pressure as they will strongly impact reproductive success (Fig 2).

3.6 Phylogenetic constraints

Phylogeny might explain flower coloration in different ways (Fig 2). First, flowers are

dependent on their genetic make-up to determine their pigments and color possibilities (Chittka

1997). The lack of bee white flowers has been associated with phylogenetic restrains (Chittka,

1999, Koski & Asman 2016). Second, related flowers can have a similar color because of their

ancestral state, if there is not enough pressure to diverge from it. Some plant families tend to have

Page 34: Marilia Fernandes Erickson...Agradeço às minhas irmãs Biny e Becky que, embora não fizeram nenhum bolo de cenoura para mim, estavam sempre com o Ifoods na mão. Aos meus pais,

30

similar colors such as Apicaceae, whose flowers vary mostly in brightness rather than hue (Chittka

1997). Evolutionary history may also affect color because it allows for similar plants to withstand

similar environmental factors, and hence to bloom close to each other. In Nepal, monocots are

more present in lower elevations, and there is more color diversity in higher elevations (Shrestha,

Dyer, Bhattarai & Burd 2014).

The phylogenetical color signal is dependent on the biochemical pathways which determine

coloration. In A. majus, single gene mutations may lead to color change in flowers (Dyer, Whitney,

Arnold, Glover & Chittka 2007). In Solanacea, biochemical pathways leading to red flowers by

anthocyanin or double production of anthocyanin and carotenoids seem to possess phylogenetical

signal (Ng & Smith 2015). More studies of floral genes should clarify whether there is convergence

or divergence in floral signals (Schiestl & Johnson 2013).

Even though some studies corroborate the importance of phylogenetic signal for flower

coloration (Reverté, Retana, Gómez & Bosch 2016; Ng & Smith 2015; Shrestha, Dyer, Bhattarai

& Burd 2014), a consensus is far from being achieved, since many studies found no effect of

phylogenetic constraints on floral color, meaning that flower color and the phylogeny are not

associated (Smith, Ané, Baum 2008; Arnold, Savolainen, Chittka 2009; McEwen & Vamosi 2010;

Gómez, Perfectti & Lorite 2015; Weber et al 2018). Many examples show that plant species are

able to produce numerous colors in their lifetimes. For example, flower color change by pollination

or age has been found in 77 families of plants that are taxonomically distinct (Weiss 1995). Plants

can also produce fruits in different coloration than flowers, which exemplify that plants can

allocate different pigments to serve desired functions (Chittka 1997). Additionally, some cultivated

flowers can come in a wide variety of colors associated with different pigments and cell structure.

Roses, for example, can be red, pink, yellow, orange, white, violet and even green (Eugster &

Page 35: Marilia Fernandes Erickson...Agradeço às minhas irmãs Biny e Becky que, embora não fizeram nenhum bolo de cenoura para mim, estavam sempre com o Ifoods na mão. Aos meus pais,

31

Märki-Fischer 1991). Adaptative radiation can also exemplify how closely related flowers can

easily diverge in color. In columbines (Aquilegia) single loss of one enzyme in the biopathway of

some anthocyanins can cause blue to red transitions in flower color (Hodges & Dering 2009). All

this indicates that given enough selective pressure, flowers are capable of rapid change in color

(Chittka 1997).

3.7 Floral age

Flower age can also affect flower color, as many plants show a dramatic color change,

different from senescence (Weiss 1995). Byrsonima variabilis A. Juss., for instance, changes

standard petal color during anthesis from yellow to orange and finally red, and bees preferentially

visits flowers with yellow standard petals when foraging for pollen (Melo, Mota, Schlindwein,

Antonini & Oliveira 2018). The retention of old flowers increases display size and, by so doing,

increases attraction of pollinators (Ishii & Sakai 2001). In fact, prolonged longevity of flowers can

increase pollination even without color change (Texido et al 2019). It seems, however, that the

retention of old flowers without color change might come with a cost, because it leads to plant-

level avoidance by pollinators that have spatial memory (Makino & Ohashi 2017).

Flower color change has been extensively associated with directing pollinators to

rewarding flowers, since flowers are unrewarding after color change (Weiss 1995). Indeed, at close

range, flower color change can direct pollinators to rewarding flowers (Sun, Liao, Xia, Guo 2005),

and is often considered an honest signal (Schaefer, Schaefer & Levey 2004, Makino & Ohashi

2017). Nevertheless, when considering long-distance attraction, it seems pollinators struggle to

differentiate the amount of rewarding or unrewarding flowers (Oberrath and Böhning-Gaese 1999,

Kudo, Ishii, Hirabayashi & Ida 2007). For this reason, flower color change may attract pollinators

at a distance via deception, by maintaining an increased display that includes unrewarding color

Page 36: Marilia Fernandes Erickson...Agradeço às minhas irmãs Biny e Becky que, embora não fizeram nenhum bolo de cenoura para mim, estavam sempre com o Ifoods na mão. Aos meus pais,

32

changed flowers that cannot be differentiated; once pollinators approach, however, it provides an

honest signal, as to which flowers are rewarding (Brito, Weynans, Sazima, Lunau 2015).

There are other benefits to the retention of old-color changed flowers as, even without

increased attraction, floral color change can decrease the amount of geitonogamous pollination

(when pollen is transferred from one flower to another flower of the same plant) (Ida & Kudo

2003, Sun, Liao, Xia, Guo 2005, Ida & Kudo 2010). In fact, flower color change seems to be such

a huge advantage that some wonder why it is not prevalent among angiosperms (Ruxton &

Schaefer 2016). Flower color change is, however, more common than it gets credit for, and often

we find new reports of color-changing flowers, even in the UV range (Ohashi, Makino & Arikawa

2015). Flower color change has evolved many times (Weiss 1995), and this outcome could be due

to simple mechanisms. Pollinators have been shown to recognize old flowers as is the case in Rosa

virginiana P.Mill., where second day flowers are paler, and bee preferentially visit younger flowers

(MacPhial, Kevan & Fuss 2007). Pigments are altered by sunlight, especially anthocyanins

(Grotewold 2006). Though color change can happen in any pigment, most color change seems to

be associated with a change in anthocyanins (Weiss 1995, Lippi, Giuliani, Gonnelli, Bini 2011).

In Viola cornuta L. flowers, changes in color are due to anthocyanins; when flowers are grown in

the dark, they do not show color change, as opposed to the white to purple change that occurs

under light conditions (Farzad, Griesbach & Weiss 2002). Evidently, flower color change has

evolved many times in relation to the natural reaction of anthocyanins to sunlight. Natural selection

would refine this natural change, inasmuch as flower color change benefits plants by attracting

more pollinators (Ishii & Sakai 2001, Ida & Kudo 2010) and diminishing geitonogamous

pollination (Ida & Kudo 2003, Sun, Liao, Xia, Guo 2005, Ida & Kudo 2010), and pollinators, by

diminishing foraging time (Kudo, Ishii, Hirabayashi & Ida 2007).

Page 37: Marilia Fernandes Erickson...Agradeço às minhas irmãs Biny e Becky que, embora não fizeram nenhum bolo de cenoura para mim, estavam sempre com o Ifoods na mão. Aos meus pais,

33

We discussed one pathway that could have led to flower color change, but there is likely to

have more than one explanation. Flower color change can be a step into transitioning flowers from

one pollinator to another, being ephemeral in evolutionary time (Ruxton & Schaefer 2016). In

Quisqualis indica (L.) DeFilipps, white flowers are mostly visited by moths and red flowers are

visited by butterflies (Yan, Wang, Sui, Wang & Zhang 2016). Phylogeny and bee-pollination can

also be major factors underlining the color change phenomenon (Ohashi, Makino & Arikawa 2015,

Makino & Ohashi 2017). There are many areas in flower color change that remain unexplored,

such as the relation of color change to cost of pigments productions, evolutionary potential, and

genetics (Ruxton & Schaefer 2016). Overall, flower color change is a phenomenon about which

much is still to be learned.

3.8 Seasonal changes in flower color

A recurring theme in the literature is that flowers of certain colors bloom at certain seasons

(Wilmer 2011). Some insects change their color preference throughout the year (Kevan 1983), and

so, flowers may bloom with the preferred color of the insects at a given time. The abundance of

insects with color preference can also change throughout the year (Kevan 1983). In Australia,

Epacris impressa Labill. (common heath) has different color morphs that vary across seasons. The

white morph is found in spring and the red flower on winter. This occurrence seems to be related

to abundance of pollinators, because birds are present in winter when the red morph blooms, and

white morphs occur in spring when insects are more plentiful (Stace & Fripp 1977).

Temperature might be a driving factor in determining floral color in polymorphic plants.

Mu, Li and Sun (2010) found that during the early flowering season when temperature was lower

and the photoperiod shorter, the white color morph of the Tibetan herb Gentiana leucomelaena

Page 38: Marilia Fernandes Erickson...Agradeço às minhas irmãs Biny e Becky que, embora não fizeram nenhum bolo de cenoura para mim, estavam sempre com o Ifoods na mão. Aos meus pais,

34

Maxim. was much more abundant that the blue morph; latter in the season, when the temperature

rose, the blue color morph became more abundant.

Flowers of darker color will be warmer than light colored flowers. Some pollinators, such

as bees, can associate color difference with warmer flowers and preferentially forage on warmer

flowers (Dyer, Whitney, Arnold, Glover & Chittka 2006). This makes for interesting pollination

systems where heat is offered as a reward. In Oncocyclus irises, pollinators do not get any nectar

or pollen reward, instead, flowers warm up quickly in the morning and so male bees who sleep

inside flowers will start foraging earlier the next day (Sapir, Shmida & Ne'eman 2005).

There is some evidence of a convergence of flower color depending on the season, but that

effect varies according to the population studied. Though the flower color of spring flowers does

not seem to be white, as previously stated (Molten 1986), the corolla color of flowers in temperate

deciduous forest, which flower in spring, tend to be lighter than non-spring flowers (Hensel &

Sargent 2012). In Germany, flower color was studied across a year period, and there was no

relationship between floral color and blooming time using bee's color category, but there was a

difference in the human color categories, showing that importance of using an ecologically

relevant visual system to analyze color (Arnold, Comber & Chittka 2009). More research,

emphasizing the pollinators perspective and accessing different populations, are needed in order

to reach a better understanding of the effects of seasons on flower coloration.

3.9 Biogeographical changes

Because different communities have different selective pressures it is also important to

study if there is a selection for specific colors depending on the habitat. Microclimates could help

explain UV patterns of flowers. Habitats with high UV-B irradiance were more likely to have UV-

absorbing flowers (Koski & Asman 2016). Another hypothesis is that flower color can vary with

Page 39: Marilia Fernandes Erickson...Agradeço às minhas irmãs Biny e Becky que, embora não fizeram nenhum bolo de cenoura para mim, estavam sempre com o Ifoods na mão. Aos meus pais,

35

different altitudes, because the amount of ambient light varies with altitude (Kevan 1983) and since

higher altitudes will have different insect visitors. When the flora of Dovrefjell–Sunndalsfjella

National Park (Germany) was studied, at different altitudes, with regard to flower coloration and

according to bee and fly vision, the results showed no effect of altitude on color (Arnold,

Savolainen, Chittka 2009). Yet, this is not always the case, as, in Nepal, flowers found in higher

altitudes show more diversity of colors than in lower ones (Shrestha, Dyer, Bhattarai & Burd

2014).

Neutral factors can also contribute to the spatial distribution of plant color morphs. The iris

Iris lutescens Lam. has two color morphs, with different distributions across Spain and France.

Different processes seem to be acting in the two regions. Spain has monomorphic populations of

either yellow or purple flowers that have little to no gene flow between them, and genetic drift

seems to be the likely factor determining the polymorphism. In France, however, there is gene

flow between populations and so, most populations are polymorphic composed of both colors

(Wang, Talavera, Min, Flaven & Imbert 2016). Similarly, in the milk thistle Silybum marianum

(L.) Gaertn., neutral process such as the founding effect and genetic drift, seem to explain the

variation of color morphs along the Mediterranean (Keasar, Gerchman & Lev-Yadum 2016).

Another factor that may yield selection for different colors is the abundance of different

kinds of pollinators across habitats. Indeed, flower coloration in Australia (Dye et al 2012) and

Israel (Chittka & Menzel 1992) seems to be shaped by Hymenoptera vision. Bees are important

pollinators in Europe, as they do not have many bird pollinators. The abundance of red flowers in

the tropics is often attributed to hummingbird pollination (Wilmer 2011). Blue-purple flowers in

the artic seem to be related to species richness of bumblebees showing a coevolution between

flower color and pollinator species (Eidesen, Little, Müller, Dickinson & Lord 2017).

Page 40: Marilia Fernandes Erickson...Agradeço às minhas irmãs Biny e Becky que, embora não fizeram nenhum bolo de cenoura para mim, estavam sempre com o Ifoods na mão. Aos meus pais,

36

Pollinators can also have differential color preferences between habitats. Bumblebees

usually have a UV-violet preference, but some populations have an additional red preference

(Raine, Ings, Dornhaus, Saleh & Chittka 2006). Hence, plants can have local adaptations

depending on pollinators. The mimetic Orchid Disa ferruginea Sw., is pollinated by a single

species of butterfly. This orchid has two color morphs occurring in different geographical regions.

The red morph occurs when there are red rewarding flowers around, and butterflies shows

preference for red flowers. Likewise, the orange morph occurs when there are orange rewarding

flowers and butterflies show orange preference (Newman, Anderson & Johnson 2012).

Some studies have tried to associate habitat with flower color, showing that there can be a

convergence of floral color, divergence in floral color, or just a random distribution. Chittka (1997)

has shown that the color distribution of flowers in a German grassland was not found to be different

from chance, but in the Brazilian rainforest flowers seemed to cluster around bee-blue. another

study, conducted in Brazil, showed that in the Restinga (a sandy area near the coast with poor soil,

the predominant vegetation is medium sized trees and shrubs) region there was a prevalence of

white flowers and in the Caatinga (a semi-arid region characterized by small thorny trees and

shrubs that shed their leaves in the summer) region most flowers were yellow (Machado & Lopes

2003). Moreover, subalpine communities in Canada show evidence divergent evolution of floral

color (McEwen & Vamosi 2010). In another study, Gumbert, Kunze and Chittka (1999) analyzed

five different habitats, within Germany, for trends in flower color. When only considering common

flowers, they did not find any prevalent color; but when rare flowers were included, the results

varied across locations, since in dry meadow and hazel shrub, plant colors were more divergent

than expected by chance. On the other hand, the same study found that in the humid meadow,

colors were more similar than expected by chance, and that in the maple forest and roadside, colors

Page 41: Marilia Fernandes Erickson...Agradeço às minhas irmãs Biny e Becky que, embora não fizeram nenhum bolo de cenoura para mim, estavam sempre com o Ifoods na mão. Aos meus pais,

37

did not differ from a random distribution. Until now, literature shows that, depending on the

habitat, there can be selection for either divergence or convergence of floral color. There are many

pressures in a given habitat that will make up their color diversity (Fig 2). In order to determine

biogeographical patterns in floral color, the evaluation of other habitats, using a pollinators

perspective, should be encouraged.

3.10 Sensory drive

According to Endler’s theory of sensory drive, the environmental bias, noise and receiver’s

sensory capabilities tend to shape the evolution of signals, by selecting signals and receivers that

better overcome noise in a given environment, selecting more conspicuous signals and more

efficient receivers (Endler 1992). In general, flower coloration is an interesting context to study

sensory drive (Schaefer, Schaefer & Levey 2004), because flowers are present in several different

environments and, since they cannot move, are restricted to the signaling conditions of the given

location. Predictively, bees prefer to forage in flowers that are more conspicuous in their

background (Forrest & Thomson 2009). We should expect the same for other pollinators inasmuch

as conspicuousness diminishes search time. More conspicuous flowers, however, would also be

more readily perceived by antagonists; and thus, natural selection against herbivory could balance

flower sexual selection for more conspicuous flowers (Fig 2).

Bees have been shown to detect changes in ambient light and use these as contextual cues

(Lotto & Chittla 2005). Filtering of ambient light in areas of abundance of woody long-lived plants,

in relation to herbaceous species, might explain why some flowers appear to have lighter corollas

(Hensel & Sargent 2012). Ambient light also varies across seasons, especially in deciduous or

semi-deciduous forests, in which the falling of the leaves will cause a different light filtering

(Endler 1993). In the understory of a green forest, we expect to have many yellow flowers, since

Page 42: Marilia Fernandes Erickson...Agradeço às minhas irmãs Biny e Becky que, embora não fizeram nenhum bolo de cenoura para mim, estavam sempre com o Ifoods na mão. Aos meus pais,

38

the canopy filters most of the red and blue light, while on the treetops, in which the broad spectrum

of the sun is found, we should expect no difference in abundance of flowers of different

colorations, except for green flowers, which would not contrast well against the green dappled

foliage.

Depending on the background contrast, the same flower may be perceived as bearing different

colors, so that pressure to overcome background noise might be crucial to development of

conspicuous colors (Bukovac, Shrestha, Garcia, Burd, Dorin & Dyer 2017). Plants that develop

dense foliage might overcome visual background noise (Bukovac, Shrestha, Garcia, Burd, Dorin

& Dyer 2017), helping bees, for instance, to forage under more visually uniform conditions

(Forrest & Thomson 2009). For flower species that occur in more than one environment (e.g. one

with dense foliage and another with thin leaves), and/or for backgrounds that go through seasonal

changes (e.g. falling leaves and leaf color changing), flower signals would also have to overcome

different background noises, that could act as important selective pressures on the evolution of

flower coloration and pollinator visual system. Nevertheless, in forests and grasslands of Germany,

according to the honeybee visual system, flowers seem to have similar colors (Binkenstein &

Schaefer 2015). The importance of background coloration to the evolution of flower visual signals

is well exemplified by a study conducted in the Eastern Mediterranean, by Dafni et al. (1999).

These authors found that there is an abundance of red flowers in the region, which are visited by

beetles (with poor red-green discrimination), not by birds (with good red-green discrimination),

because flowers bloom before the green foliage develops, enhancing the red flower contrast against

the sandy background (Wilmer 2011).

Sensory drive can play an important role explaining the convergent evolution of coloration

across seasons and habitats, as environmental noise can make flowers of a certain color less

Page 43: Marilia Fernandes Erickson...Agradeço às minhas irmãs Biny e Becky que, embora não fizeram nenhum bolo de cenoura para mim, estavam sempre com o Ifoods na mão. Aos meus pais,

39

conspicuous, and thus less visited by pollinators. According to sensory drive, we should expect

flowers in similar signaling environments to evolve towards similar colors. This is not considered

in many of the studies trying to determine floral coloration in different localities and seasons, as

they do not attempt to control different background colors or light environments. How sensory

drive affects the evolution of color is an increasingly popular topic, however, only recently we

have been observing the use of plant models to study concepts used, almost, exclusively in animal

communication.

3.11 Prospects

As we have seen, several different factors affect flower coloration (Fig 2), and different

environmental pressures will determine if it is best to diverge or converge signals with their

neighbors. While, plant coloration is dependent on available pigments, a direct pigment-pollinator

link, is unlikely. Pigments are determined by phylogeny (Chittka, Shmida, Troje & Menzel 1994),

but small mutations on plant pigments may cause perceptual change in flower color (Dyer,

Whitney, Arnold, Glover & Chittka 2007) and plant linages often display transition in color

(McEwen & Vamosi 2010). Flower colors are determined by natural selection, which favors colors

which increase plant fitness against multiple selective pressures, but neutral selection such as

genetic drift and founding effect can also explain certain patterns in flower color. Flower coloration

offers a unique perspective in interspecific communication. Yet studies that account for other

visitors other than pollinators are rare, and it is a growing field where there is much to be done.

Page 44: Marilia Fernandes Erickson...Agradeço às minhas irmãs Biny e Becky que, embora não fizeram nenhum bolo de cenoura para mim, estavam sempre com o Ifoods na mão. Aos meus pais,

40

Figure 2. Diagram exemplifying how flower color is affected by different factors discussed in this

review.

Studies involving geographical and temporal patterns need to be conducted in more

environments so we can better understand the relationship between location and flower color.

Sensory drive in flower coloration can be an interesting topic that has much to be explored. It is

important to conduct research in relation to the coloration of flowers across forest canopy. In future

studies of plant coloration, it is also important to consider herbivory and other antagonistic

interactions and how they shape the evolution of flower color. Future studies in the field can focus

on many questions. How are eavesdroppers shaping the evolution of flower color? How often do

plants use color signals to repel visitors? Should we still use floral syndromes as the bases of

flower-pollinator interactions? If not, how does pollinator preference shape the evolution of floral

signals? Which biochemical pathways lead to flower color change? What environmental pressures

shape color in flowers? Are flowers converging or diverging in color in a given habitat? How are

all these factors related to the evolutionary history of these plants?

Page 45: Marilia Fernandes Erickson...Agradeço às minhas irmãs Biny e Becky que, embora não fizeram nenhum bolo de cenoura para mim, estavam sempre com o Ifoods na mão. Aos meus pais,

41

3.12 References

Arikawa, K. (2017). The Eyes and Vision of Butterflies. Journal of Physiology, 595(16), 5457–

5464. https://doi.org/10.1113/JP273917

Armbruster, W. S. (2002). Can Indirect Selection and Genetic Context Contribute to Trait

Diversification? A Transition-Probability Study of Blossom-Colour Evolution in Two

Genera. Journal of Evolutionary Biology, 15(3), 468–486.

https://doi.org/10.1046/j.1420-9101.2002.00399.x

Arnold, S., Le Comber, S., & Chittka, L. (2009). Flower Color Phenology in European Grassland

and Woodland Habitats, Through the Eyes of Pollinators. Israel Journal of Plant

Sciences, 57, 211–230. https://doi.org/10.1560/IJPS.57.3.211

Arnold, S., Savolainen, V., & Chittka, L. (2009). Flower Colours Along an Alpine Altitude

Gradient, Seen Through the Eyes of Fly and Bee Pollinators. Arthropod-Plant

Interactions, 3, 27–43. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11829-009-9056-9

Binkenstein, J., & Schaefer, H. M. (2015). Flower Colours in Temperate Forest and Grassland

Habitats: A Comparative Study. Arthropod-Plant Interactions, 3(9), 289–299.

https://doi.org/10.1007/s11829-015-9369-9

Briscoe, A. D., & Chittka, L. (2001). The Evolution of Color Vision in Insects. Annual Review

of Entomology, 46(1), 471–510. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.ento.46.1.471

Brito, V., Weynans, n, Sazima, M., & Lunau, K. (2015). Trees as Huge Flowers and Flowers as

Oversized Floral Guides: The Role of Floral Color Change and Retention of Old Flowers

in Tibouchina pulchra. Frontiers in Plant Science, 6(352).

https://doi.org/doi:10.3389/fpls.2015.00362

Bukovac, Z., Shrestha, M. R., Garcia, J. E., Burd, M., Dorin, A., & Dyer, A. G. (2017). Why

Background Colour Matters to Bees and Flowers. Journal of Comparative Physiology A,

203(5), 369–380. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00359-017-1175-7

Brembs, B., & Hempel de Ibarra, N. (2006). Different Parameters Support Generalization and

Discrimination Learning in Drosophila at The Flight Simulator. Learning & Memory,

13(5), 629–637. https://doi.org/10.1101/lm.319406

Burger, J., & Edwards, G. E. (1996). Photosynthetic Efficiency, and Photodamage by UV and

Visible Radiation, in Red versus Green Leaf Coleus Varieties. Plant and Cell

Physiology, 37(3), 395–399. https://doi.org/10.1093/oxfordjournals.pcp.a028959

Chalker-Scott, L. (1999) Environmental Significance of Anthocyanins in Plant Stress Responses.

Photochemistry and Photobiology, 70(1): 1-9

Page 46: Marilia Fernandes Erickson...Agradeço às minhas irmãs Biny e Becky que, embora não fizeram nenhum bolo de cenoura para mim, estavam sempre com o Ifoods na mão. Aos meus pais,

42

Chittka, L. (1997). Bee Color Vision Is Optimal for Coding Flower Color, but Flower Colors

Are Not Optimal for Being Coded—Why? Israel Journal of Plant Sciences, 45(2–3),

115–127. https://doi.org/10.1080/07929978.1997.10676678

Chittka, L. (1999). Bees, White Flowers, and the Color Hexagon - A Reassessment? No, Not

Yet. Naturwissenschaften, 86, 595–597. https://doi.org/10.1007/s001140050681

Chittka, L., & Menzel, R. (1992). The Evolutionary Adaptation of Flower Colours and the Insect

Pollinators’ Colour Vision. Journal of Comparative Physiology, 171(2), 171–181.

https://doi.org/doi:10.1007/bf00188925

Chittka, L., Shmida, A., Troje, N., & Menzel, R. (1994). Ultraviolet as a Component of Flower

Reflections, and the Colour Perception of Hymenoptera. Vision Research, 34(11), 1489

-1508.

Chittka, L., Thomson, J. D., & Waser, N. M. (1999). Flower Constancy, Insect Psychology, and

Plant Evolution. Naturwissenschaften, 86(8), 361–377.

https://doi.org/10.1007/s001140050636

Cuthill, I. C., Allen, W. L., Arbuckle, K., Caspers, B., Chaplin, G., Hauber, M. E., … Caro, T.

(2017). The Biology of Color. Science, 357(6350).

https://doi.org/10.1126/science.aan0221

Dafni, A. M., Bernhardt, P., Shmida, A., Ivri, Y., Greenbaum, S., O’Toole, C., & Losito, L. (1990).

Israel Journal of Botany, 39:1-2, 81-92 https://doi.org/1080/0021213X.1990.10677134

de Melo, B. T., Mota, T., Schlindwein, C., Antonini, Y., & Oliveira, R. (2018). Floral Colour

Change in Byrsonima variabilis (Malpighiaceae) as a Visual Cue for Pollen but Not Oil

Foraging by Oil-Collecting Bees. Science of Nature, 105(7), 46.

https://doi.org/10.1007/s00114-018-1572-y

Dyer, A. G. (2012). The Mysterious Cognitive Abilities of Bees: Why Models of Visual Processing

Need to Consider Experience and Individual Differences in Animal Performance. Journal

of Experimental Biology, 215(3), 387–395. https://doi.org/10.1242/jeb.038190

Dyer, A. G., Whitney, H. M., Arnold, S. E.J., Glover, B. J., & Chittka, L. (2006). Bees Associate

Warmth with Floral Colour. Nature, 442(7102), 525. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/442525a

Dyer, A. G., Whitney, H. M., Arnold, S. E. J., Glover, B. J., & Chittka, L. (2007). Mutations

Perturbing Petal Cell Shape and Anthocyanin Synthesis Influence Bumblebee Perception

of Antirrhinum majus Flower Colour. Arthropod-Plant Interactions, 1(1), 45–55.

https://doi.org/10.1007/s11829-007-9002-7

Dyer Adrian G., Boyd-Gerny Skye, McLoughlin Stephen, Rosa Marcello G. P., Simonov Vera,

& Wong Bob B. M. (2012). Parallel Evolution of Angiosperm Colour Signals: Common

Evolutionary Pressures Linked to Hymenopteran Vision. Proceedings of the Royal Society

B, 279(1742), 3606–3615. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2012.0827

Page 47: Marilia Fernandes Erickson...Agradeço às minhas irmãs Biny e Becky que, embora não fizeram nenhum bolo de cenoura para mim, estavam sempre com o Ifoods na mão. Aos meus pais,

43

Eidesen, P. B., Little, L., Müller, E., Dickinson, K. J. M., & Lord, J. M. (2017). Plant–pollinator

interactions affect colonization efficiency: abundance of blue-purple flowers is correlated

with species richness of bumblebees in the Arctic. Biological Journal of the Linnean

Society, 121(1), 150–162. https://doi.org/10.1093/biolinnean/blw006

Endler, J. A. (1992). Signals, Signal Conditions, and the Direction of Evolution. American

Naturalist, 139, S125–S153. https://doi.org/10.1086/285308

Endler, J. A. (1993). The Color of Light in Forests and Its Implications. Ecological Monographs,

63(1), 1–27.

Eugster, C. H., & Märki‐Fischer, E. (1991). The Chemistry of Rose Pigments. Angewandte

Chemie, 30(6), 654–672. https://doi.org/10.1002/anie.199106541

Farzad, M., Griesbach, R., & Weiss, M. R. (2002). Floral Color Change in Viola cornuta L.

(Violaceae): A Model System to Study Regulation of Anthocyanin Production. Plant

Science, 162, 225–231. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0168-9452(01)00557-X

Fenster, C. B., Armbruster, W. S., Wilson, P., Dudash, M. R., & Thomson, J. D. (2004).

Pollination Syndromes and Floral Specialization. Annual Review of Ecology,

Evolution, and Systematics, 35(1), 375–403.

https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.ecolsys.34.011802.132347

Forrest, J., & Thomson, J. D. (2009). Background Complexity Affects Colour Preference in

Bumblebees. Naturwissenschaften, 96(8), 921–925.

https://doi.org/10.1007/s00114-009-0549-2

Fukada-Tanaka, S., Inagaki, Y., Yamaguchi, T., Saito, N., & Iida, S. (2000). Colour-Enhancing

Protein in Blue Petals. Nature, 407(6804), 581. https://doi.org/10.1038/35036683

Gawryszewski, F. M. (2018) Color vision models: Some simulations, a general n‐dimensional

model, and the colourvision R package. Ecology and Evolution, 8 (16), 8158-8170.

https://doi.org/10.1002/ece3.4288

Giurfa, M., Vorobyev, M., Kevan, P., & Menzel, R. (1996). Detection of Coloured Stimuli by

Honeybees: Minimum Visual Angles and Receptor Specific Contrasts. Journal of

Comparative Physiology, 178, 699–709. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00227381

Glover, B. J., Whitney, H. M. (2010) Structural Colour and Iridescence in Plants: The Poorly

Studied Relations of Pigment Colour. Annals of Botany, 105, 505-511

https://doi.org/10.1093/aob/mcq007

Glover, B. J., & Martin, C. (1998). The Role of Petal Cell Shape and Pigmentation in Pollination

Success in Antirrhinum majus. Heredity, 80, 778–784.

https://doi.org/10.1046/j.13652540.1998.00345.x

Page 48: Marilia Fernandes Erickson...Agradeço às minhas irmãs Biny e Becky que, embora não fizeram nenhum bolo de cenoura para mim, estavam sempre com o Ifoods na mão. Aos meus pais,

44

Gould, K. S. (2004). Nature’s Swiss Army Knife: The Diverse Protective Roles of Anthocyanins

in Leaves. Journal of Biomedicine and Biotechnology, 2004(5), 314–320.

http://doi.org/10.1155/s1110724304406147

Gómez, J. M., Perfectti, F., & Lorite, J. (2015). The Role of Pollinators in Floral Diversification

in a Clade of Generalist Flowers. Evolution, 69(4), 863–878.

https://doi.org/10.1111/evo.12632

Grotewold, E. (2006). The Genetics and Biochemistry of Floral Pigments. Annual Review of

Plant Biology, 57(1), 761–780.

https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.arplant.57.032905.105248

Gumbert A., Kunze J., & Chittka L. (1999). Floral Colour Diversity in Plant Communities, Bee

Colour Space and a Null Model. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B, 266(1429),

1711–1716. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.1999.0836

Guzmán, B., Gómez, J. M., & Vargas, P. (2017). Is Floral Morphology a Good Predictor of Floral

Visitors to Antirrhineae (Snapdragons And Relatives)? Plant Biology, 19(4), 515–524.

https://doi.org/10.1111/plb.12567

Gawryszewski, F. M. (2018) Color vision models: Some simulations, a general n‐dimensional

model, and the colourvision R package. Ecology and Evolution. 8: 8159– 8170.

https://doi.org/10.1002/ece3.4288

Heinrich, B. (1975). Bee Flowers: A Hypothesis on Flower Variety and Blooming Times.

Evolution, 29(2), 325–334. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1558-5646.1975.tb00212.x

Hensel, L. E., & Sargent, R. D. (2012a). A Phylogenetic Analysis of Trait Convergence in the

Spring Flora. Botany, 90(7), 557–564. https://doi.org/10.1139/b2012-029

Herrera, G., Zagal, J. C., Diaz, M., Fernández, M. J., Vielma, A., Cure, M., … Palacios, A. G.

(2008). Spectral Sensitivities of Photoreceptors and Their Role in Colour Discrimination

in the Green-backed Firecrown Hummingbird (Sephanoides sephaniodes). Journal of

Comparative Physiolog A, 194(9), 785–794. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00359-008-0349-8

Hodges, S. A., Derieg, N. J. (2009) Adaptative Radiations: From Field to Genomic Studies. PNAS,

106 (Supplement 1) 9947-9954. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0901594106

Ida, T. Y., & Kudo, G. (2003). Floral Color Change in Weigela middendorffiana

(Caprifoliaceae): Reduction of Geitonogamous Pollination by Bumble Bees. American

Journal of Botany, 90(12), 1751–1757. https://doi.org/10.3732/ajb.90.12.1751

Ida, T. Y., & Kudo, G. (2010). Modification of Bumblebee Behavior by Floral Color Change and

Implications for Pollen Transfer in Weigela middendorffiana. Evolutionary Ecology, 24,

671–684. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10682-009-9324-2

Page 49: Marilia Fernandes Erickson...Agradeço às minhas irmãs Biny e Becky que, embora não fizeram nenhum bolo de cenoura para mim, estavam sempre com o Ifoods na mão. Aos meus pais,

45

Irwin, R. E., & Brody, A. K. (2000). Consequences of Nectar Robbing for Realized Male

Function in a Hummingbird-Pollinated Plant. Ecology, 81(9), 2637–2643.

https://doi.org/10.2307/177481

Irwin, R. E., Strauss, S. Y., Storz, S., Emerson, A., & Guibert, G. (2003). The Role of

Herbivores in the Maintenance of a Flower Color Polymorphism in Wild Radish.

Ecology, 84(7), 1733–1743.

Ishii, H. S., & Sakai, S. (2001). Effects of Display Size and Position on Individual Floral Longevity

in Racemes of Narthecium asiaticum (Liliaceae). Functional Ecology, 15(3), 396–405.

https://doi.org/10.1046/j.1365-2435.2001.00536.x

Johnson, E., Berhow, M., & F Dowd, P. (2008). Colored and White Sectors From Star-Patterned

Petunia Flowers Display Differential Resistance to Corn Earworm and Cabbage Looper

Larvae. Journal of Chemical Ecology, 34, 757–765.

https://doi.org/10.1007/s10886-008-9444-0

Johnson, K. A. (2013). Are There Pollination Syndromes in the Australian Epacrids (Ericaceae:

Styphelioideae)? A Novel Statistical Method to Identify Key Floral Traits per Syndrome.

Annals of Botany, 112(1), 141–149. https://doi.org/10.1093/aob/mct105

Kelber, A. (2016). Colour Vision: Random Retina of Butterflies Explained. Current Biology,

26(19), R900–R902. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2016.08.018

Kelber, A., Balkenius, A., & Warrant, E. J. (2003). Colour Vision in Diurnal and Nocturnal

Hawkmoths. Integrative and Comparative Biology, 43(4), 571–579.

https://doi.org/10.1093/icb/43.4.571

Keasar, T., Gerchman, Y., & Lev-Yadun, S. (2016). A seven-year study of flower-color

polymorphism in a Mediterranean annual plant. Basic and Applied Ecology, 17(8), 741–

750. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.baae.2016.10.003

Kevan, P. (1983). Floral Colors Through the Insect Eye: What They are and What They Mean. In:

Handbook of Experimental Biology (pp. 3–30). Scientific and Academic Editions.

Kootstra, A. (1994). Protection From UV-B-induced DNA Damage by Flavonoids. Plant

Molecular Biology, 26(2), 771–774. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00013762

Koski, M. H. and Ashman, T. (2016), Macroevolutionary patterns of ultraviolet floral

pigmentation explained by geography and associated bioclimatic factors. New Phytol, 211:

708-718. https://doi.org/10.1111/nph.13921

Kudo, G., Ishii, H. S., Hirabayashi, Y., & Ida, T. Y. (2007). A Test of the Effect of Floral Color

Change on Pollination Effectiveness Using Artificial Inflorescences Visited by

Bumblebees. Oecologia, 154(1), 119–128. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00442-007-0820-1

Kuniyasu, M., Yumoto, T., Nagamitsu, T., Makoto, K., Nagamasu, H., Sakai, S., … Tamiji, I.

(1998). Pollination Biology in a Lowland Dipterocarp Forest in Sarawak, Malaysia. I.

Page 50: Marilia Fernandes Erickson...Agradeço às minhas irmãs Biny e Becky que, embora não fizeram nenhum bolo de cenoura para mim, estavam sempre com o Ifoods na mão. Aos meus pais,

46

Characteristics of the Plant-Pollinator Community in a Lowland Dipterocarp Forest.

American Journal of Botany, 85, 1477–1501. https://doi.org/10.2307/2446404

Lev-Yadun, S., & Gould, K. S. (2007). What do Red and Yellow Autumn Leaves Signal.

Botanical Review, 73(4), 279–289.

https://doi.org/10.1663/00068101(2007)73[279:WDRAYA]2.0.CO;2

Lev-Yadun, S., Ne’eman, G., & Keasar, T. (2017). Differences in Flower Colors between Spiny

and Non-spiny Asteraceae Species: A Possible Case of Aposematism? Flora, 239, 98

103. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.flora.2017.12.002

Lippi, M. M., Giuliani, C., Gonnelli, T., & Bini, L. M. (2011). Floral Color Changes in Boswellia

sacra Flueck. (Burseraceae): A Dialogue Between Plant and Pollinator. Flora, 206(9), 821

826. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.flora.2011.04.008

Lotto, R. B., & Chittka, L. (2005). Seeing the light: Illumination as a Contextual Cue to Color

Choice Behavior in Bumblebees. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences,

102(10), 3852–3856. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0500681102

Lunau, K., & Wester, P. (2017). Mimicry and Deception in Pollination. Chapter ten In G. Becard

(ed.), Advances in Botanical Research (pp. 259–279).

https://doi.org/10.1016/bs.abr.2016.10.005

Lunau, Klaus. (2014). Visual Ecology of Flies with Particular Reference to Colour Vision and

Colour Preferences. Journal of Comparative Physiology A, 200(6), 497–512.

https://doi.org/10.1007/s00359-014-0895-1

Lunau, Klaus, Papiorek, S., Eltz, T., & Sazima, M. (2011). Avoidance of Achromatic Colours

by Bees Provides a Private Niche For Hummingbirds. Journal of Experimental Biology,

214(9), 1607–1612. https://doi.org/10.1242/jeb.052688

Machado, I. C. & Lopes, A. V. (2003). Recursos Florais e Sistemas de Polinização e Sexuais em

Caatinga. In I. R. Leal, M. Tabarelli, J. M. C. DaSilva (Eds.), Ecologia e conservação da caatinga.

(pp. 515 -564 ) Recife, Ed. Universitaria da UFPE.

MacPhail, V. J., Kevan, P. G., & Fuss, C. (2007). Effect of Flower Age on Honey Bee Behavior

When Rewards are Kept Constant. Journal of Apicultural Research, 46(4), 293–294.

https://doi.org/10.1080/00218839.2007.11101411

Makino, T. T., & Ohashi, K. (2017). Honest Signals to Maintain a Long-Lasting Relationship:

Floral Colour Change Prevents Plant-Level Avoidance by Experienced Pollinators.

Functional Ecology, 31(4), 831–837. https://doi.org/10.1111/1365-2435.12802

Mayfield, M. M., Waser, N. M., & Price, M. V. (2001). Exploring the ‘Most Effective Pollinator

Principle’ with Complex Flowers: Bumblebees and Ipomopsis aggregata. Annals of

Botany, 88(4), 591–596. https://doi.org/10.1006/anbo.2001.1500

Page 51: Marilia Fernandes Erickson...Agradeço às minhas irmãs Biny e Becky que, embora não fizeram nenhum bolo de cenoura para mim, estavam sempre com o Ifoods na mão. Aos meus pais,

47

McEwen Jamie R., & Vamosi Jana C. (2010). Floral Colour Versus Phylogeny in Structuring

Subalpine Flowering Communities. Proceedings of the Royal Society B, 277(1696),

2957-2965. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2010.0501

Molten, A. F. (1986). Pollination Ecology of the Spring Wildflower Community of a Temperate

Deciduous Forest. Ecological Monographs, 56(1), 21–42.

https://doi.org/doi:10.2307/2937269

Mori, M., Yoshida, K., Ishigaki, Y., Matsunaga, T., Nikaido, O., Kameda, K., & Kondo, T.

(2005). UV-B Protective Effect of a Polyacylated Anthocyanin, HBA, in Flower Petals of

the Blue Morning Glory, Ipomoea tricolor Cv. Heavenly Blue. Bioorganic & Medicinal

Chemistry, 13(6), 2015–2020. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bmc.2005.01.011

Muchhala, N., & Thomson, J. D. (2012). Interspecific Competition in Pollination Systems: Costs

to Male Fitness Via Pollen Misplacement. Functional Ecology, 26(2), 476–482.

https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-2435.2011.01950.x

Mu, J., Li, G., & Sun, S. (2010). Petal Color, Flower Temperature, and Behavior in an Alpine

Annual Herb, Gentiana leucomelaena (Gentianaceae). Arctic, Antarctic, and Alpine

Research, 42(2), 219–226. https://doi.org/10.1657/1938-4246-42.2.219

Müller, B., Glösmann, M., Peichl, L., Knop, G. C., Hagemann, C., & Ammermüller, J. (2009).

Bat Eyes Have Ultraviolet-Sensitive Cone Photoreceptors. PLOS ONE, 4(7), e6390.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0006390

Newman, E., Anderson, B., & Johnson, S. D. (2012) Flower Colour Adaptation in a Mimetic

Orchid. Proceeding of the Royal Society B, 279 (1737) 2309-2313

https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2011.2375

Ng, J., & Smith, S. D. (2016). Widespread Flower Color Convergence in Solanaceae via Alternate

Biochemical Pathways. New Phytologist, 209(1), 407–417.

https://doi.org/10.1111/nph.13576

Niu, Y., Sun, H., & Stevens, M. (2018). Plant Camouflage: Ecology, Evolution, and

Implications. Trends in Ecology & Evolution, 33(8), 608–618.

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tree.2018.05.010

Oberrath, R., & Böhning-Gaese, K. (1999). Floral Color Change and the Attraction of Insect

Pollinators in Lungwort (Pulmonaria collina). Oecologia, 121(3), 383–391.

https://doi.org/10.1007/s004420050943

Ohashi, K., Makino, T. T., & Arikawa, K. (2015). Floral Colour Change in the Eyes of

Pollinators: Testing Possible Constraints and Correlated Evolution. Functional Ecology,

29(9), 1144–1155. https://doi.org/10.1111/1365-2435.12420

Ollerton, J., Alarcón, R., Waser, N. M., Price, M. V., Watts, S., Cranmer, L., … Rotenberry, J.

(2009). A Global Test of the Pollination Syndrome Hypothesis. Annals of Botany, 103(9),

1471–1480. https://doi.org/10.1093/aob/mcp031

Page 52: Marilia Fernandes Erickson...Agradeço às minhas irmãs Biny e Becky que, embora não fizeram nenhum bolo de cenoura para mim, estavam sempre com o Ifoods na mão. Aos meus pais,

48

Ollerton, J., Winfree, R., & Tarrant, S. (2011). How Many Flowering Plants are Pollinated by

Animals? Oikos, 120(3), 321–326. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1600-0706.2010.18644.x

Olsson, P., Lind, O., & Kelber, A. (2018). Chromatica Achromatic Vision: Parameter Choice

and Limitations for Reliable Model Predictions. Behavioral Ecology, 29(2), 273–282.

https://doi.org/10.1093/beheco/arx133

Papiorek, S., Rohde, K., & Lunau, K. (2013). Bees’ Subtle Colour Preferences: How Bees Respond

to Small Changes in Pigment Concentration. Naturwissenschaften, 100(7), 633–643.

https://doi.org/10.1007/s00114-013-1060-3

Peitsch, D., Fietz, A., Hertel, H., de Souza, J., Ventura, D. F., & Menzel, R. (1992). The Spectral

Input Systems of Hymenopteran Insects and their Receptor-Based Colour Vision. Journal

of Comparative Physiology A, 170(1), 23–40.

Peter, C. I., & Johnson, S. D. (2008). Mimics and Magnets: The Importance of Color and

Ecological Facilitation in Floral Deception. Ecology, 89(6), 1583–1595.

Pourcel, L., Routaboul, J., Cheynier, V., Lepiniec, L., & Debeaujon, I. (2007). Flavonoid oxidation

in plants: from biochemical properties to physiological functions. Trends in Plant Science, 12(1),

29–36. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.tplants.2006.11.006

Raine, Ings, Dornhaus, Saleh, & Chittka. (2006). Adaptation, Genetic Drift, Pleiotropy, and

History in the Evolution of Bee Foraging Behavior. Advances in the Study of Behavior.

https://doi.org/10.1016/S0065-3454(06)36007-X

Renoult, J. P., Kelber, A., & Schaefer, H. M. (2017). Colour Spaces in Ecology and Evolutionary

Biology. Biological Reviews, 92(1), 292–315. https://doi.org/10.1111/brv.12230

Reverté, S., Retana, J., Gómez, J. M., & Bosch, J. (2016). Pollinators Show Flower Colour

Preferences but Flowers with Similar Colours do not Attract Similar Pollinators. Annals of

Botany, 118(2), 249–257. https://doi.org/10.1093/aob/mcw103

Roy, B. A., & Raguso, R. A. (1997). Olfactory Versus Visual Cues in a Floral Mimicry System.

Oecologia, 109(3), 414–426. https://doi.org/10.1007/s004420050101

Ruxton, G. D., & Schaefer, H. M. (2016). Floral Colour Change as a Potential Signal to

Pollinators. Current Opinion in Plant Biology, 32, 96–100.

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pbi.2016.06.021

Sapir, Y., Shmida, A., & Ne’eman, G. (2005). Morning floral heat as a reward to the pollinators

of the Oncocyclus irises. Oecologia, 147(1), 53–59. doi:10.1007/s00442-005-0246-6

Schaefer, H. M., & Rolshausen, G. (2006). Plants on Red alert: Do Insects Pay Attention?

BioEssay, 28(1), 65–71. https://doi.org/10.1002/bies.20340

Page 53: Marilia Fernandes Erickson...Agradeço às minhas irmãs Biny e Becky que, embora não fizeram nenhum bolo de cenoura para mim, estavam sempre com o Ifoods na mão. Aos meus pais,

49

Schaefer, H. M., Schaefer, V., & Levey, D. J. (2004). How Plant-Animal Interactions Signal New

Insights in Communication. Trends in Ecology and Evolution, 19(11), 577–584.

https://doi.org/doi:10.1016/j.tree.2004.08.003

Schemske, D. W., & Horvitz, C. C. (1984). Variation Among Floral Visitors in Pollination

Ability: A Precondition for Mutualism Specialization. Science, 225(4661), 519–521.

https://doi.org/10.1126/science.225.4661.519

Schiestl, F. P., & Johnson, S. D. (2013). Pollinator-Mediated Evolution of Floral Signals. Trends

in Ecology & Evolution, 28(5), 307–315. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tree.2013.01.019

Shrestha, M., Dyer, A. G., Bhattarai, P., & Burd, M. (2014). Flower Colour and Phylogeny Along

an Altitudinal Gradient in the Himalayas of Nepal. Journal of Ecology, 102(1), 126–135.

https://doi.org/10.1111/1365-2745.12185

Shuttleworth, A., & Johnson, S. D. (2009). A Key Role for Floral Scent in a Wasp-Pollination

System In Eucomis (Hyacinthaceae). Annals of Botany, 103(5), 715–725.

https://doi.org/10.1093/aob/mcn261

Smith, S. D., Ané, C., & Baum, D. A. (2008). The Role of Pollinator Shifts in the Floral

Diversification of Iochroma (solanaceae). Evolution, 62(4), 793–806.

https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1558-5646.2008.00327.x

Stace, H. M., & Fripp, Y. J. (1977). Raciation In Epacris impressa. II. Habitat Differences and

Flowering Times. Australian Journal of Botany, 25(3), 315–323.

https://doi.org/10.1071/bt9770315

Stanton, M. L. (1987). Reproductive Biology of Petal Color Variants in Wild Populations of

Raphanus Sativus: I. Pollinator Response to Color Morphs. American Journal of Botany,

74(2), 178–187. https://doi.org/10.1002/j.1537-2197.1987.tb08595.x

Stevens, M. (2013). Sensory Ecology, Behavior, & Evolution. Oxford: Oxford university press.

Stevens, M., Stoddard, M. C., & Higham, J. P. (2009). Studying Primate Color: Towards Visual

System-Dependent Methods. International Journal of Primatology, 30(6), 893–917.

https://doi.org/10.1007/s10764-009-9356-z

Sun, S. G., Liao, K., Xia, J., & Guo, Y. H. (2005). Floral Colour Change in Pedicularis

monbeigiana (Orobanchaceae). Plant Systematics and Evolution, 255(1), 77–85.

https://doi.org/10.1007/s00606-005-0348-y

Teixido, A. L., Duarte, M. O., Ballego‐Campos, I., Sanín, D., Cunha, J. S., Oliveira, C. S., &

Silveira, F. a. O. (2019). One for All and All for One: Retention Of Colour-Unchanged Old

Flowers Increases Pollinator Attraction in a Hermaphroditic Plant. Plant Biology, 21(1),

167–175. https://doi.org/10.1111/plb.12906

Troje, N. (1993). Spectral Categories in the Learning Behaviour of Blowflies. Zeitschrift Für

Naturforschung C, 48(1–2), 96–104. https://doi.org/10.1515/znc-1993-1-218

Page 54: Marilia Fernandes Erickson...Agradeço às minhas irmãs Biny e Becky que, embora não fizeram nenhum bolo de cenoura para mim, estavam sempre com o Ifoods na mão. Aos meus pais,

50

Wang, H., Talavera, M., Min, Y., Flaven, E., & Imbert, E. (2016). Neutral processes contribute to

patterns of spatial variation for flower colour in the Mediterranean Iris lutescens

(Iridaceae). Annals of Botany, 117(6), 995–1007. doi:10.1093/aob/mcw036

Waser, N. M. (1986). Flower Constancy: Definition, Cause, and Measurement. American

Naturalist, 127(5), 593–603. https://doi.org/DOI: 10.1086/284507

Waser, N. M., Chittka, L., Prince, M. V., Williams, N. M., & Ollerton, J. (1996). Generalization

in Pollination Systems, and Why it Matters. Ecology, 77(4), 1043–1060.

https://doi.org/DOI: 10.2307/2265575

Weber, M. G., Cacho, N. I., Phan, M. J. Q., Disbrow, C., Ramírez, S. R., & Strauss, S. (2018).

The Evolution of Floral Signals in Relation to Range Overlap in a Clade of California

Jewelflowers (Streptanthus s.l.). Evolution, 72(4), 798–807.

https://doi.org/10.1111/evo.13456

Weiss, M. R. (1995). Floral Color Change: A Widespread Functional Convergence. American

Journal of Botany, 82(2), 167–185. https://doi.org/10.2307/2445525

Westerkamp, C. (996). Pollen in Bee‐Flower Relations Some Considerations on Melittophily*.

Botanica Acta, 109, 325–332. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1438-8677.1996.tb00580.x

White, T. E., Dalrymple, R. L., Herberstein, M. E., & Kemp, D. J. (2017). The Perceptual

Similarity of Orb-Spider Prey Lures and Flower Colours. Evolutionary Ecology, 31(1), 1

20. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10682-016-9876-x

Whitney, H. M., Kolle, M., Andrew, P., Chittka, L., Steiner, U., & Glover, B. J. (2009). Floral

Iridescence, Produced by Diffractive Optics, Acts as a Cue for Animal Pollinators.

Science, 323(5910), 130–133. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1166256

Wilmer, P. (2011). Pollination and Floral Ecology. Princeton: Pinceton University Press.

Yan, J., Wang, G., Sui, Y., Wang, M., & Zhang, L. (2016). Pollinator Responses to Floral Colour

Change, Nectar, and Scent Promote Reproductive Fitness in Quisqualis indica

(Combretaceae). Scientific Reports, 6, 24408. https://doi.org/10.1038/srep24408

Page 55: Marilia Fernandes Erickson...Agradeço às minhas irmãs Biny e Becky que, embora não fizeram nenhum bolo de cenoura para mim, estavam sempre com o Ifoods na mão. Aos meus pais,

51

4. ARTIGO 2

Pollination syndromes do not predict flower conspicuousness by different

pollinators

Page 56: Marilia Fernandes Erickson...Agradeço às minhas irmãs Biny e Becky que, embora não fizeram nenhum bolo de cenoura para mim, estavam sempre com o Ifoods na mão. Aos meus pais,

52

Pollination syndromes do not predict flower conspicuousness by different pollinators

Marilia Erickson1 , Diogo Jackson de Aquino Silva & Daniel M. A. Pessoa

Laboratory of Sensory Ecology, Department of Physiology and Behavior, Universidade Federal do Rio Grande

do Norte. Natal – RN, Brazil. CEP 59078-970

2- MSc student; E-mail: [email protected]

4.1 Abstract

Pollination syndromes have long been used to categorize and study flowers. Recently, this idea

came into question as, it seems, most pollinators and flowers are generalist. There is a debate about

whether we should continue to use pollination syndromes to study pollination. Little empirical data

has been adduced to explain why pollinators prefer characteristics described by pollination

syndromes. The aim of this study is to contribute to the ongoing debate, by investigating if flower

conspicuousness, through the eyes of bees, flies, butterflies and hummingbirds, can explain color

preferences described by traditional pollination syndromes. We used the Receptor Noise Limited

model to calculate chromatic contrast between flowers and background. First, we tested if

pollinators could discriminate better flowers of their own syndrome when compared with other

floral syndromes. Second, we tested if flowers with colors associated with pollination syndromes

were more conspicuous to their pollinators. And finally, we compared if pollinators could detect

better flowers or colors of their own syndromes when compared with other pollinators. We found

that pollinators do not see flowers of their own syndrome as more conspicuous, when compared

to flowers of other syndromes, and that the colors of the most conspicuous flowers were not those

predicted by pollination syndromes. On average, animals with tetrachromatic vision had a higher

color contrast that trichromatic animals. Pollinators, however, could detect well all flower colors,

with the exception that bees saw red poorly, as previously described in the literature. Overall, our

findings support the idea that flowers are generalist regarding pollinators. This does not, however,

mean that color preferences do not exist, as preferences could be explained by other mechanisms,

such as innate preference, hue or brightness.

Key words: Color vision; floral biology; flower color; Apis mellifera; Drosophila melanogaster;

Heliconius erato; Sephanoides sephanoides.

Page 57: Marilia Fernandes Erickson...Agradeço às minhas irmãs Biny e Becky que, embora não fizeram nenhum bolo de cenoura para mim, estavam sempre com o Ifoods na mão. Aos meus pais,

53

4.2 Introduction

Pollinator pressure and sexual selection are considered to be the main factors driving the

evolution of floral characteristics (Schiestl & Johnson 2013). Since pollinators with similar

morphology exercise similar pressures on flowers, there is a tendency for flowers to converge

towards certain traits (Fenster, Armbruster, Wilson, Dudash & Thomson 2004). Flower species

often converge to characteristics associated with groups of pollinators giving rise to the idea of

pollination syndromes, which is often used to predict pollinators of given flowers (Wilmer 2011).

By looking at flower morphology, it would then be possible, on this account, to characterize its

pollinator, and, indeed, some papers have been able to show that pollination syndromes have

compelling predictive power. Rosas-Guerrero et al (2014) conducted a meta-analysis of pollination

data, finding pollinators that matched floral syndromes to be more effective pollinators. In

Australian epacrids, pollination syndromes characteristics correctly identified bird pollinated

species in 86% of cases, fly pollinated species in 78% of cases and bee pollinated species in 69%

of cases (Johnson 2013). While in South African flora, floral syndromes correctly predicted

pollinators in 82% of the time (Johnson & Wester 2017). This is not, however, always the case,

for many researchers have shown little support for pollination syndromes (Waser, Chittka, Prince,

Williams & Ollerton 1996; Ollerton et al 2009; Hernández-Yáñez, Lara-Rodríguez, Díaz-

Castelazo, Dáttilo & Rico-Gra 2013).

For one thing, pollination syndromes have been criticized on the grounds that the

relationship between flower and pollinator is neither as peaceful nor as clear-cut as previously

supposed (Waser, Chittka, Prince, Williams & Ollerton 1996). In fact, flowers employ pollinators

to transport their gametes while pollinators are foraging for resources, which relationship could be

described, at best, as mutual exploitation (Westerkamp 1996). Furthermore, most flowers receive

Page 58: Marilia Fernandes Erickson...Agradeço às minhas irmãs Biny e Becky que, embora não fizeram nenhum bolo de cenoura para mim, estavam sempre com o Ifoods na mão. Aos meus pais,

54

visits from multiple pollinators, and pollinators often visit more than one flower, consequently

most flowers tend to be generalist regarding pollinators (Waser, Chittka, Prince, Williams &

Ollerton 1996). Regardless of the widespread use of pollination syndromes, it is still unknown how

well syndromes describe phenotypic variation for plant-pollinator interaction (Ollerton, Rech,

Waser & Prince 2015), such as flower shape, nectar amount, color, time of anthesis, presence of

nectar guides, among others (Wilmer 2011).

Color is one of the main characteristics attributed to pollinator syndromes (Faegri & Van

der Pijl 1979), and it assures long distance attraction and flower constancy (Chittka & Menzel

1992). Flowers that are more conspicuous are preferred by pollinators because they diminish

search time and increase foraging efficiency (Forrest & Thomson 2009). Red flowers, for example,

have been shown to be more conspicuous for hummingbirds than white, yellow or orange ones

(Herrera et al 2008). Attracting naive pollinators due to their innate color preferences can limit the

evolution of flower color and lead to convergent evolution as explained by pollination syndromes

(Lunau & Maier 1995). Bumblebees have innate preference for flowers in the violet-blue range

(Briscoe & Chittka 2001). Innate preference can also reflect adaptation to pollen feeding, as is the

case with the hoverfly Eristalis tenax, which shows the innate reaction of extending their proboscis

in response to yellow stimulus (Lunau & Wacht 1994). Reverté, Retana, Gómez & Bosch (2016)

found that pollinators groups prefer to visit flowers of similar color (i.e; bees visit purple flowers,

butterflies visit pink flowers). The relationship between innate preferences of pollinators is often

used to justify the convergence of floral signals, but this supposition often lacks empirical data

(Schiestl & Johnson 2013). For instance, bee flowers come in almost all possible colors, not only

pink, purple, blue, white and yellow, as predicted by their pollination syndrome (Wilmer 2011).

Page 59: Marilia Fernandes Erickson...Agradeço às minhas irmãs Biny e Becky que, embora não fizeram nenhum bolo de cenoura para mim, estavam sempre com o Ifoods na mão. Aos meus pais,

55

Perhaps this is due to pollinators ready capacity to associate flower color and reward, not being

limited by innate preferences (Weiss 1997, Gumbert 2000).

Flower syndrome colors are attributed considering human vision, even though flowers can

reflect ultraviolet (UV) light. This poses a problem, since humans and pollinators differ regarding

their color vision. Bees are trichromats with photoreceptors in the UV, blue and green range

(Peitsch et al 1992). Flies, butterflies and hummingbirds often see the full electromagnetic

spectrum with receptors on the UV, blue, green, and red region; but even within these groups, peak

receptor sensitivities vary (Briscoe & Chittka 2001; Herrera et al 2008; McCulloch, Osorio &

Briscoe 2016). Although it is stated that bees prefer flowers that reflect UV light, all major groups

of pollinators can also see UV light. The preference for UV reflecting flowers depends on context.

Red flowers pollinated by orchid bees usually reflect UV while white orchid flowers pollinated by

bees usually lack UV reflection; the opposite is true for hummingbirds (Lunau, Papiorek, Eltz &

Sazima 2011).

The investigation of flower coloration from the perspective of multiple color visual systems

has been largely underexplored (Schaefer, Schaefer & Levey 2004). Considering this, here we test

if the color preference observed in pollination syndromes is sustained by certain flower colors

being more conspicuous to pollinators. First, we test the hypothesis that pollinators detect flowers

of their own syndrome better than flowers of other syndromes. Then, we investigate what color is

more conspicuous to each pollinator (bees, flies, butterflies and hummingbirds). We predict that

bees will see blue, yellow and white flowers best; birds will see red flowers best; butterflies will

see red and yellow flowers best; while flies will see green and white flowers best, since these are

the colors associated with each pollination syndrome (Wilmer 2011). Third, we want to compare

conspicuousness between pollinators. We expect that, for instance, the bees’ visual system should

Page 60: Marilia Fernandes Erickson...Agradeço às minhas irmãs Biny e Becky que, embora não fizeram nenhum bolo de cenoura para mim, estavam sempre com o Ifoods na mão. Aos meus pais,

56

find bee flowers more conspicuous than flowers pollinated by flies, butterflies and hummingbirds,

and so on. We also expect that certain flower colors should be more conspicuous to a given

pollinator, according to color preferences described in each pollination syndrome. As in, bees

should outperform birds in the detection of blue, and birds should outperform bees in the detection

of red. To test these hypotheses, we calculate the chromatic contrast between flowers and their

backgrounds, according to the visual system of each pollinator, using the Receptor Noise Limited

model (Osorio & Vorobyev 1996).

4.3 Materials and methods

4.3.1. Data collection

We collected flowers between February of 2018 and January of 2019, in two different sites

in Northeast of Brazil: the Floresta Nacional de Assu (-5,579745;-36,942139), which corresponds

to a Caatinga (xeric shrubland/thorn forest) region, and the Reserva Biologica de Guaribas (-

6,74117;-35,138377), which corresponds to an Atlantic forest region. We collected flowers at the

understory vegetation, that were up to 1.5m of the ground, which comprised mostly herbs and

shrubs. After collection, flowers (Table S1) were transported, as quick as possible (maximum

delay of one and a half hours), to an improvised laboratory. We used a USB4000-UV-VIS

spectrometer connected to a DH-2000-BAL light source and a bifurcated QR450-7-XSR fiber (all

by Ocean Optics Inc.), and software SpectraSuite (Ocean Optics Inc.), to measure the flowers and

their respective backgrounds (leaves, sand, tree trunks, and modified leaves of the inflorescence,

Table S1). In order to measure flowers that were, at least, one millimeter in diameter, we attached

a custom-made probe holder at the end of the fiber, to taper the measurable surface area to 1 mm.

Reflectance spectra registered with the custom-made probe holder, 3D printed in black plastic,

were very similar to those registered with a RPH-1 probe holder (Ocean Optics Inc.) (Fig S1). All

Page 61: Marilia Fernandes Erickson...Agradeço às minhas irmãs Biny e Becky que, embora não fizeram nenhum bolo de cenoura para mim, estavam sempre com o Ifoods na mão. Aos meus pais,

57

stimuli were measured at with the probe holder in direct contact to the object surface, allowing

measurements to be taken at a 90° angle, with a constant distance of 5mm from the probe. To

calibrate the equipment, we used a Spectralon Reflectance Standard WS-1-SL (Ocean Optics Inc.)

as the white standard, then turned the light source off and obstructed the probe holder orifice with

a black cloth, for determining the black standard. In total, we measured 44 species from the

Caatinga and 50 species from the Atlantic forest habitat. Since two of the flowers from the

Caatinga were polymorphic regarding color (Croton sp. and Jacquemontia pentanthos) we set the

total number of targets at 96 flower specimens. For those species whose flowers changed color,

we used the pre-change color to characterize the species (i.e. for Lantana camara we used yellow

flowers which precede orange and red floral phases). All flowers were mounted in Exsiccatae,

deposited in the herbarium of Parque das Dunas (RN), and were identified by a professional

botanist.

To measure the ambient light, we used the same spectrophotometer described above,

attached to an QP450-2-XSR optic fiber (Ocean optics), with a cosine corrector (CC-3-UV-S,

Ocean Optics, Inc.). The apparatus was calibrated using a LS-1-CAL calibration lamp (Ocean

Optics, Inc.). The optic fiber was pointed upwards towards the sky, at each of the desired areas, to

acquire illuminance measurements (Fig S2). For forest flowers we did not measure the illuminant,

but instead used the forest shade illuminant, available in “pavo2” package.

4.3.2. Categorization of flower pollination syndrome and flower coloration

To categorize each flower as belonging to a pollination syndrome, we used the characters

described by Wilmer (2011), that could be assessed either visually or olfactorily (“main color”,

“nectar guides”, “scent”, “shape”, “nectar site” and “pollen deposited”). Our flowers fell into five

Page 62: Marilia Fernandes Erickson...Agradeço às minhas irmãs Biny e Becky que, embora não fizeram nenhum bolo de cenoura para mim, estavam sempre com o Ifoods na mão. Aos meus pais,

58

syndromes: melittophily- Bee pollinated (51), psychophily – butterfly pollinated (23), myophily-

fly pollinated (19), ornithophily – bird pollinated (2) and anemophily – wind pollinated (1).

Flower color was categorized based on Wilmer (2011) categories, to which we created

individual descriptions (Table S2). Because of the reduced sample size of each category, we

grouped the categories of Table 1 in new ones (Table 1). For the tests we used these new categories.

To characterize if flowers had UV, we created a standardized method by comparing different

techniques (Percentage of reflectance between 300nm and 400nm compared to reflectance at

exactly the wavelength 350nm in different ratios 5%, 8% and 10%). The criteria we found more

reliable (matched the noticeable peaks in the spectrum) was if reflectance had at least 10% of

reflectance in the Wavelength 350nm, so we used it to characterize flowers in our study. Therefore

for each color category we had flowers without reflectance on UV (UV-), flowers with reflectance

on the UV (UV +) and flowers with and without together (Total) to represent how humans have

categorized flowers in syndromes.

Table 1. New color categories with description and number of flowers in each group.

Color category Description Includes

Number of flowers sampled

UV- UV+ Total

Red Pure red, unsaturated red, dark red, and

red with tints of yellow red, mauve,

orange, brown 4 4 8

Yellow Pure yellow, light yellow and muted

yellow cream, yellow, dull

beige 18 11 29

Green All greens, yellow with tints of blue, and

blues with tints of yellow green, greenish,

pale green 10 3 13 Blue pure blue and blues tinged red blue, pink, purple 13 3 16

white true white, off-whites and light grays white, dull white,

dull 25 5 30 Mottled irregular patches Mottled 0 0 0

Page 63: Marilia Fernandes Erickson...Agradeço às minhas irmãs Biny e Becky que, embora não fizeram nenhum bolo de cenoura para mim, estavam sempre com o Ifoods na mão. Aos meus pais,

59

4.3.3. Visual modeling

To calculate the color difference between the background and each flower petal we used

the Receptor Noise Limited (RNL) model (Osorio & Vorobyev 1996), which gives the chromatic

contrast (S) in Just Noticeable Difference (JND) units. For the RNL model, the detectability

threshold is 1 JND, which means that targets contrasting, solely in color, from background

elements in less than 1 JND should not be distinguishable by the visual system taken into

consideration. Different pollinators might have different detectability thresholds, however, since

these data are not available for most species (Olsson, Lind, & Kelber, 2018), we used the standard

threshold of 1 JND for all models. The higher the chromatic contrast (measured in JND units), the

higher the color difference between surfaces (Ham & Osorio 2007) and hence they would be more

easily detected. We classified flowers as cryptic (S < 1 JND), barely detectable (S between 1

and 3 JND) and detectable (S > 3 JND).

We used the software R studio and the “pavo2” package (Maia, Bitton, Doucet & Shawkey

2018) for all the visual modeling analyses. The input from each visual system is described in Table

2. All spectral reflectance was smoothed in ‘pavo2’ package with a spawn of 0.12 prior to

modeling. The illuminant spectra were chosen according to the habitats in which each flower was

most frequently found. We used forest shade illuminant, available in “pavo2” package, for flowers

found inside the forest. For flowers found in the Caatinga, or outside the forest (in the Restinga),

we used illuminants that we previously gathered at each area.

Regarding the proportion of photoreceptors, we attributed a value of one to the least

frequent photoreceptor, while scaling the other frequencies accordingly (i.e; 1:1:3 for bees). Since

the frequency of receptors for short (S) and middle (M) wavelenghts are known to vary in bees

(Chittka & Raine 2006), we assumed each type had equal proportions. For S. sephanoides

Page 64: Marilia Fernandes Erickson...Agradeço às minhas irmãs Biny e Becky que, embora não fizeram nenhum bolo de cenoura para mim, estavam sempre com o Ifoods na mão. Aos meus pais,

60

hummingbirds, since this information is unavailable, we used the average proportion of

photoceptors found in passerines (Heart & Hunt 2007). H. erato butterflies exhibit a visual sexual

dimorphism (McCulloch, Osorio & Briscoe 2016), so we modelled female (F) and male (M) vision

separately

Table 2. Parameters used in our visual modeling.

Animal model

Popular name

Photoreceptors’ peak sensitivities Photoceptor proportion Weber fraction

Apis mellifera Honey bee 328, 436, 532 nm1 S: 1; M: 1; L: 35 0.128

Heliconius erato F

Butterfly 355, 390, 470, 555 nm2 UV: 1.3; S: 1; M: 2.4; L: 14.32 0.059 Heliconius erato

M Butterfly

390, 470, 555 nm2 S: 1; M: 1.5; L: 7.72 0.059 Drosophila

melanogaster House fly

345, 375, 437, 508 nm3 UV: 1; S: 2; M: 1; L: 2 6 0.1010 Sephanoides sephanoides

Humming bird 371, 444, 508, 560 nm4 UV: 1; S: 1,9; M: 2,8; L: 3,3 7 0.1010

1- Peitschet al 1992; 2- McCulloch, Osorio & Briscoe 2016; 3- Salcedo et al. 1999; 4- Herrera et al 2008; 5- Chittka

& Raine 2006; 6- Kirschfeld & Franceschini 1978; 7- Heart & Hunt 2007; 8- Hempel de Ibarra, Giurfa & Vorobyev

2001; 9- Koshitaka, Kinoshita, Vorobyev & Arikawa 2008; 10- Vorobyev, Osorio, Bennett, Marshall, & Cuthill

1998.

4.3.4. Statistical Analysis

We used grouped flowers in previously described color and syndrome categories to test

compare color contrast of flower within a specific visual system, and between different pollinators

visual systems. For comparisons within a pollinator visual system, we tested if pollinator groups

could detect better flowers bellowing to a syndrome (i.e; bees saw mellitiphilous flowers better

than psychofilous flowers and myophilous flowers) and if in each of the syndrome categories

flowers were more conspicuous to the respective pollinator of each category (i.e, melittophily

flowers more conspicuous for Apis melifera, myophily flowers more conspicuous to Drosophila

melanogaster). Comparisons between visual systems were made in three ways. First, in order to

Page 65: Marilia Fernandes Erickson...Agradeço às minhas irmãs Biny e Becky que, embora não fizeram nenhum bolo de cenoura para mim, estavam sempre com o Ifoods na mão. Aos meus pais,

61

compare if any specific pollinator saw all flowers as more conspicuous than other pollinators we

checked if the color contrast of all flowers together was higher to a specific pollinator (i.e; If overall

flowers were more conspicuous to bees, flies, butterflies or birds). Second, we tested which color

had higher color contrast by each pollinator (i.e; If bees see blue flowers better than red flowers).

Third, which pollinator saw each color better (i.e; If birds saw red flowers better than bees). To

test our hypothesis, we used the software R studio. To do this we first check the distribution of the

data. Most data did not have a normal distribution (Shapiro-Wilk, p-valor < 0.05), so we used a

Kruskal-Wallis analysis, with Dunn’s test as a post-hoc analysis, adjusting the P value through

Bonferroni’s correction. Some of the groups had fewer than 5 species per category and therefore

statistics would be unreliable, so we only show statistical results for samples higher than five.

Considering the UV reflection made little difference in the overall results, probably because it

lowers the number of flowers in each category, so we only present the summed results of each

color category (UV- plus UV+).

4.4. Results

For flies, butterflies (either males and females) and hummingbirds all chromatic contrasts

(S) between flowers and backgrounds were above the predetermined detection treshhold (JND >

1), meaning all flowers were detectable for the visual systems of these pollinators. (Table 4) Yet,

for bees, two flowers (from a total of 96) of the red category (one mauve and one orange) felt

below the detection threshold, meaning that these two flowers would not be distinguished from

their background by the visual system of bees (Table 4).

Table 3. Number of flowers, of different colors, that are cryptic (S < 1 JND), barely detectable

(S between 1 and 3 JND) and detectable (S > 3 JND), according to the visual system of bees,

flies, butterflies (males and females) and hummingbirds.

Page 66: Marilia Fernandes Erickson...Agradeço às minhas irmãs Biny e Becky que, embora não fizeram nenhum bolo de cenoura para mim, estavam sempre com o Ifoods na mão. Aos meus pais,

62

Statistically, considering the visual system of flies (x2 =2.67, df =2, P = 0.26), female

butterflies (x2 =2.86, df =2, P = 0.23), male butterflies (x2 =2.46, df =2, P = 0.29), hummingbirds

(x2 =2.37, df =2, P = 0.3), and bees (x2 =4.27, df =2, P = 0.11) flowers of different pollination

syndromes were not significantly different. In other words, flowers from each pollination

syndrome were equally conspicuous, irrespective of the visual system considered (Figure 1). The

sample size of bird pollinated flowers was to small to be compared statistically.

Modeled Spcies Color

∆S < 1 JND 1 JND < ∆S < 3 JND 3 JND < ∆S

Blue 3 13

Green 7 6

Yellow 6 23

Red 2 2 4

White 4 26

Blue 16

Green 1 12

Yellow 29

Red 3 3

White 30

Blue 16

Green 1 12

Yellow 29

Red 1 7

White 1 29

Blue 2 14

Green 4 9

Yellow 2 27

Red 1 7

White 3 27

Blue 16

Green 5 8

Yellow 2 27

Red 1 7

White 3 27S. sephanoides

Discriminability

A. mellifera

D. melanogaster

H. erato

H. erato

Page 67: Marilia Fernandes Erickson...Agradeço às minhas irmãs Biny e Becky que, embora não fizeram nenhum bolo de cenoura para mim, estavam sempre com o Ifoods na mão. Aos meus pais,

63

Figure 1. Boxplot showing distribution of chromatic contrast values between flowers and

background elements, comparison is within visual system of pollinators where flowers are grouped

Page 68: Marilia Fernandes Erickson...Agradeço às minhas irmãs Biny e Becky que, embora não fizeram nenhum bolo de cenoura para mim, estavam sempre com o Ifoods na mão. Aos meus pais,

64

by syndrome A) flies, B) Female Butterfly, C) Bee, D) Male butterfly, E) Hummingbird. Different

letters indicate statistical difference between groups. (α= 0.05).

Regarding flowers of different color categories (Fig 2)., bees saw better white and red was

the least conspicuous color (x2 = 17.34, df =4, P < 0.05). Flies also detected white flowers better

and detected red and green flowers worst (x2 =16.68, df =4, P < 0.05). For female butterflies, even

if our analysis showed there was a difference in chromatic contrast between colors (x2 = 11.14, df

=4, P = 0.03), the post hoc failed to detect where is the difference and we will treat these results as

all colors being equally conspicuous. For male butterflies, however, yellow was the most

conspicuous color, and white and green were least conspicuous (x2 =13.58, df =4, P = 0.01).

Lastly, for humming-birds yellow was the most conspicuous color and red was the least

conspicuous (x2 =11.6, df =4, P = 0.02).

When comparing chromatic contrasts of all flowers as seen by different pollinators, D.

melanogaster and female H. erato had the highest chromatic contrasts, followed by S. sephanoides,

male H. erato and A. mellifera (x2 = 100.17, df = 4, P < 0.05).

Continuing comparisons between pollinators, flowers predicted by each syndrome were

not better viewed by their assumed Pollinators (Fig 3). Bee pollinated flowers were best vied by

flies and female butterflies, rather than by bees (x2 = 63.92, df =4, P < 0.05). Fly pollinated flowers

(x2 = 16.55, df =4, P < 0.05) and Butterfly pollinated flowers (x2 = 30.8, df =4, P < 0.05) were best

viewed by both D. melanogaster and female H. erato as seems to be the regular pattern. Samples

sizes of both ornotophilous (2) and anemophilous (1) flowers were too small to allow statistical

comparison, however, we identified a pattern suggesting that ornotophilous flowers could be best

seen by S. sephanoides.

Page 69: Marilia Fernandes Erickson...Agradeço às minhas irmãs Biny e Becky que, embora não fizeram nenhum bolo de cenoura para mim, estavam sempre com o Ifoods na mão. Aos meus pais,

65

Figure 2. Boxplot showing distribution of chromatic contrast values between flowers and

background elements, comparison is within visual system of pollinators where flowers are grouped

by color. A) flies, B) Female Butterfly, C) Bee, D) Male butterfly, E) Hummingbird. Different

letters indicate statistical difference between groups. (α= 0.05).

Page 70: Marilia Fernandes Erickson...Agradeço às minhas irmãs Biny e Becky que, embora não fizeram nenhum bolo de cenoura para mim, estavam sempre com o Ifoods na mão. Aos meus pais,

66

Figure 3. Boxplot showing distribution of chromatic contrast values between flowers and

background elements, comparison is within pollination syndrome where data are grouped by visual

system of pollinators. Abbreviations are the following AM) A.mellifera, DM) D. melanogaster,

HE♀) Female H. erato, HE♂) Male Heliconius erato SS) S. sephanoides. Different letters indicate

statistical difference between groups. (α= 0.05).

Page 71: Marilia Fernandes Erickson...Agradeço às minhas irmãs Biny e Becky que, embora não fizeram nenhum bolo de cenoura para mim, estavam sempre com o Ifoods na mão. Aos meus pais,

67

Figure 4. Boxplot showing distribution of chromatic contrast values between flowers and

background elements, comparison is within flower coloration where data are grouped by visual

system of pollinators. Abbreviations are the following AM) A. mellifera, DM) D. melanogaster,

HE♀) Female H. erato, HE♂) Male H. erato SS) S. sephanoides. Different letters indicate

statistical difference between groups. (α= 0.05).

Page 72: Marilia Fernandes Erickson...Agradeço às minhas irmãs Biny e Becky que, embora não fizeram nenhum bolo de cenoura para mim, estavam sempre com o Ifoods na mão. Aos meus pais,

68

When comparing colors between pollinators, color categories blue: (x2 = 22.51 , df =4, P <

0.05), green (x2 = 11.02 , df =4, P = 0.02), white ( x2 = 51.38 , df =4, P < 0.05), yellow (x2 =

34.587 , df =4, P < 0.05) followed the general trend of having a higher chromatic contrast for D.

melanogaster and Femle H. erato followed by Male H. erato and S. sephanoides, with A. mellifera

having the lowest contrast in all cases (Fig 4). Although red was not statistically significant (x2 =

8.62, df =4, P = 0.07), still followed the trend graphically (Fig 4).

4.5. Discussion

Comparison within pollinators showed that pollinators could not identify flowers of their

own syndrome better, so the first hypothesis was not corroborated. Flowers signaling equally to

all pollinators would be good evidence that they are generalist regarding pollination as previously

stated (Waser, Chittka, Prince, Williams & Ollerton 1996).

Isolating the color factor of pollination syndrome could also not support color preferences

previously described in the literature. White flowers would be the most conspicuous to flies, but

not green. Previous studies have shown some flies have innate reactions to yellow (Lunau & Wacht

1994), but these results do not show much relation with color contrast. there are little evidential

bases to explain why flies would prefer colors stated in pollination syndromes.

For female butterflies all colors were highly conspicuous and tests failed to see where the

difference in color detection is. For male butterfly, however, there was a clear difference between

the detection of yellow and other colors. In fact, male butterflies could detect yellow flowers as

well as tetrachromats. In Heliconius butterflies, color vision likely evolved due to sexual selection

and the need to find mates (Briscoe et al 2010). Many butterflies in this genus have yellow wing

patterns, and yellow and ultraviolet are important mating colors for Heliconius (Finkbeiner,

Fishman, Osorio & Briscoe 2017). Remarkably, even butterflies that do not visit flowers have

Page 73: Marilia Fernandes Erickson...Agradeço às minhas irmãs Biny e Becky que, embora não fizeram nenhum bolo de cenoura para mim, estavam sempre com o Ifoods na mão. Aos meus pais,

69

innate preferences for yellow flowers (Weiss 1997; Balamrali, Edison, Somanathan &

Kodandaramaiah 2019). So, the preference for yellow could be linked to the role that coloration

has in sexual selection. Hence, yellow flowers could be exploiting these butterflies’ sensory bias

in order to assure their pollination. Reverté, Retana, Gómez & Bosch (2016), however, found

butterfly preference for pink flowers, while Weiss (1997) found secondary innate bias for blue and

purple flowers, which colors are typically associated with bee pollinated flowers.

For birds, our results show that red was the least conspicuous color, which is unexpected

considering that red is the color typically associated with bird pollinated flowers. The red in the

ornithophily syndrome, however, has been associated with avoidance by bees, not by a specific

preference for red by birds (Lunau, Papiorek, Eltz & Sazima 2011), and according to our model,

bees indeed would have difficulty detecting red flowers. As they could not differentiate two of

the red flowers from their background colors. There result also contrast with the ones found by

Herrera et al (2008), where red was the most conspicuous color to S. sephanoides. This could be

related to our reduced sample size or due to the fact that we did not find any purely red flowers.

Lastly, for bees red was the least contrasting color, but still does not corroborate the

previously described bee colors as green was also relatively conspicuous. According to the

literature, bees have an innate color preference for UV-blue and blue-green flowers (Giufa, Núñez,

Chittka & Menzel 1995). White flowers were, however the most conspicuous to bees, so bee

preference for UV does not seem to be related with conspicuousness. When comparing which

colors were most conspicuous to each pollinator, pattern did not follow the one described in

pollination syndromes Therefore, our second hypothesis is not corroborated.

When comparing between species, D. melanogaster (housefly) was consistently the animal

model with highest chromatic contrast, and therefore would better detect flowers. Fly color vision

Page 74: Marilia Fernandes Erickson...Agradeço às minhas irmãs Biny e Becky que, embora não fizeram nenhum bolo de cenoura para mim, estavam sempre com o Ifoods na mão. Aos meus pais,

70

is still not well studied and understood, and only a few species were tested in regard to color vision

(Lunau 2014). Hoverflies seem to have a categorical visual system being able to discriminate

colors between categories, but not within categories (Troje 1993). There is, however, criticism to

this model due to insufficient research (White, Dalrymple, Herberstein & Kemp 2017). For

instance, D. melanogaster, the species we chose to use in our models, has been shown to detect

colors within the same category (Brembs & de Ibarra 2006). The visual ecology of flies is still a

growing topic (Lunau 2014) and hence chromatic contrast might not be the most appropriate tool

to be used. Females Butterflies had a higher chromatic contrast than males, which is expected,

because males are trichromats. Despite being tetrachromat, hummingbirds had an intermediate

color vision between flies and female butterflies, and bees and male butterflies, which are

trichromats. This could be due to the model parameters used to compare between pollinators, as

small changes in parameters can cause big changes in JND (Olsson, Lind, & Kelber, 2018). It is

often stated that the evolution of flower color is shaped by pollinator preference exercised by bees

(Dyer et al 2012). Yet we found that bees had the poorest color vision among all flower visitors

modelled. Bees only use color vision to detect flower at close range, using the achromatic channels

at a distance (Giufa, Vorobyev, Kevan & Menzel 1996), so there is little need for a higher color

contrast at a distance. General background materials cluster in the perceptual space of bees and are

all achromatic, appearing gray to bees (Chittka, Shmida, Troje & Menzel 1994). That facilitates

the detection of color targets independent on the chromatic contrast.

Regarding pollination syndromes; fly flowers were best seen by flies, but flies saw all other

flowers better as well. Psychophily flowers were best seen by female butterflies, but not by males.

Bird flowers (Ornithophylous) were more conspicuous for hummingbirds, but a larger sample size

is required for evaluating this hypothesis statistically. Bee flowers were less conspicuous for bees,

Page 75: Marilia Fernandes Erickson...Agradeço às minhas irmãs Biny e Becky que, embora não fizeram nenhum bolo de cenoura para mim, estavam sempre com o Ifoods na mão. Aos meus pais,

71

as the general pattern was for bees to have lowest chromatic contrast. Color conspicuousness also

followed the general trend of tetrachromats having a higher contrast than trichromats. Since both

divisions by pollination syndromes and by color followed the general pattern of flies having higher

chromatic contrast followed by female butterflies, hummingbird, male butterflies and bees; we do

not consider the hypothesis that pollinators would detect flowers of their respective syndrome

better than other pollinators or that pollinators would detect colors related to their syndrome better

than other pollinators corroborated.

In this study, we did not find any empirical evidence that sustains pollination syndromes.

Colors in pollination syndromes could be explained by other mechanisms, such as innate

preference. Yet pollinators can easily associate color and reward, overcoming their initial biases

(Giufa, Núñez, Chittka & Menzel 1995, Weiss 1997). Innate preferences may also be adapted to

local flora (Raine & Chittka 2007). Therefore, it is difficult for innate color preferences to play a

key role in pollination syndromes. Reverté, Retana, Gómez & Bosch (2016) found that, despite

pollinator’s preference for flowers of certain colors, flower color does not dictate pollinator

assembly. Other works has shown no association between colors and pollination syndromes

(Kuniyasu et al 1998, Hernández-Yáñez, Lara-Rodríguez, Díaz-Castelazo, Dáttilo, Rico-Gray

2013).

Despite pollination syndromes importance in pollination studies, they might not be an

accurate way to group flowers (Ollerton et al 2009). And overall, our results show that plants seem

to be generalist when signaling for pollinators and pollinators can easily detect most flowers,

corroborating that generalization is the rule and not the exception in pollination systems (Waser,

Chittka, Prince, Williams & Ollerton 1996, Hernández-Yáñez, Lara-Rodríguez, Díaz-Castelazo,

Dáttilo, Rico-Gray 2013, Reverté, Retana, Gómez & Bosch 2016). In order to maintain flower

Page 76: Marilia Fernandes Erickson...Agradeço às minhas irmãs Biny e Becky que, embora não fizeram nenhum bolo de cenoura para mim, estavam sempre com o Ifoods na mão. Aos meus pais,

72

constancy, flowers are under pressure to diverge in color (Waser 1986, Schaefer, Schaefer & Levey

2004). That could explain the limited number of existing pollinator groups, and a much greater

diversity of flower colors and morphologies (Heinrich 1975), divergence seems to be the

prevailing strategy. Furthermore, pollinator abundancy also varies across time, so pollination

networks are opportunistic rather than pre-determined by plant morphology (Alarcón, Waser &

Ollerton 2008), so maintaining ambiguous characteristics would assure attraction of secondary

pollinators.

4.6. Conclusion

Flowers were not more conspicuous to their pollinators and more conspicuous flower

colors were not the ones prescribed by pollination syndromes. Tetrachromat pollinators detected

flowers better than trichromat pollinators. Accordingly, the question of why pollination syndromes

are associated with flower colors remains. Contrast is only one method for analyzing color; and it

is possible that other elements such as hue or brightness also play a key role in determining

pollinator’s preference for colors. An analysis with a larger sample could further help determine

how conspicuousness plays a role in flower-pollinator interactions.

Page 77: Marilia Fernandes Erickson...Agradeço às minhas irmãs Biny e Becky que, embora não fizeram nenhum bolo de cenoura para mim, estavam sempre com o Ifoods na mão. Aos meus pais,

73

4.7. References

Alarcón, R., Waser, N. M., & Ollerton, J. (2008). Year-To-Year Variation in the Topology of a Plant-

Pollinator Interaction Network. Oikos, 117, 1796–1807. https://doi.org/doi: 10.1111/j.0030-

1299.2008.16987.x

Balamurali, G. S., Edison, A., Somanathan, H., & Kodandaramaiah, U. (2019). Spontaneous Colour

Preferences and Colour Learning in the Fruit-Feeding Butterfly, Mycalesis mineus. Behavioral

Ecology and Sociobiology, 73(3). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00265-019-2648-1

Brembs, B., & Hempel de Ibarra, N. (2006). Different Parameters Support Generalization and

Discrimination Learning in Drosophila at the Flight Simulator. Learn Memory, 13, 629–637.

Briscoe, A. D., Bybee, S. M., Bernard, G. D., Yuan, F., Sison-Mangus, M. P., Reed, R. D., … Chiao,

C. C. (2010). Positive Selection of a Duplicated UV-Sensitive Visual Pigment Coincides with

Wing Pigment Evolution in Heliconius Butterflies. Proceedings of the National Academy of

Sciences, 107(8), 3628–3633. https://doi.org/doi:10.1073/pnas.0910085107

Briscoe, A. D., & Chittka, L. (2001). The Evolution of Color Vision in Insects. Annual Review of

Entomology, 46, 471–510.

Chittka, L., & Menzel, R. (1992). The Evolutionary Adaptation of Flower Colours and the Insect

Pollinators’ Colour Vision. Journal of Comparative Physiology, 171(2), 171–181.

https://doi.org/doi:10.1007/bf00188925

Chittka, L., & Raine, N. E. (2006). Recognition of Flowers by Pollinators. Current Opinion in Plant

Biology, 9, 428–435. https://doi.org/DOI 10.1016/j.pbi.2006.05.002

Chittka, L., Shmida, A., Troje, N., & Menzel, R. (1994). Ultraviolet as a Component of Flower

Reflections, and the Colour Perception of Hymenoptera. Vision Research, 34(11), 1489–1508.

Dyer, A. G., Boyd-Gerny, S., McLoughlin, S., Rosa, M. G., Simonov, V., & Wong, B. B. (2012).

Parallel Evolution of Angiosperm Colour Signals: Common Evolutionary Pressures Linked to

Hymenopteran Vision. Proceeding of the Royal Society B, 279(1742), 3606-36–15.

https://doi.org/doi: 10.1098/rspb.2012.0827

Faergri, K., & Van der Pijl. (1979). The Principles of Pollination Ecology. 3rd ed.. Oxford: Pergamon

Press.

Fenster, C. B., Armbruster, W. S., Wilson, P., Dudash, M. R., & Thomson, J. D. (2004). Pollination

Syndromes and Floral Specialization. Annual Review of Ecology, Evolution, and Systematics,

35(1), 375–403. https://doi.org/doi:10.1146/annurev.ecolsys.34.011802.132347

Finkbeiner, S. D., Fishman, D., A., Osorio, D., & Briscoe, A. D. (2017). Ultraviolet and Yellow

Reflectance but not Fluorescence is Important for Visual Discrimination of Conspecifics by

Heliconius erato. Journal of Experimental Biology, 153593. https://doi.org/doi:

10.1242/jeb.153593

Page 78: Marilia Fernandes Erickson...Agradeço às minhas irmãs Biny e Becky que, embora não fizeram nenhum bolo de cenoura para mim, estavam sempre com o Ifoods na mão. Aos meus pais,

74

Forrest, J., & Thomson, J. D. (2009). Background Complexity Affects Colour Preference in

Bumblebees. Naturwissenschaften, 96, 921–925. https://doi.org/DOI 10.1007/s00114-009-0549-2

Giufa, M., Núñez, J., Chittka, L., & Menzel, R. (1995). Colour Preferences of Flower-Naive Honeybees.

Journal of Comparative Physiology A, 177(3), 247–259. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00192415

Giufa, M., Vorobyev, M., Kevan, P., & Menzel, R. (1996). Detection of Coloured Stimuli by

Honeybees: Minimum Visual Angles and Receptor Specific Contrasts. Journal of Comparative

Physiology A, 178(5), 699–709. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00227381

Gumbert, A. (2000). Color Choices by Bumble Bees (Bombus terrestris): Innate Preferences and

Generalization After Learning. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 48, 36–43.

Ham, A. D., & Osorio, D. (2007). Colour Preferences and Colour Vision in Poultry Chicks. Proceedings

of the Royal Society B, 274(1621), 1941–1948. https://doi.org/doi:10.1098/rspb.2007.0538

Hart, N. S., & Hunt, D. M. (2007). Avian Visual Pigments: Characteristics, Spectral Tuning, and

Evolution. The American Naturalist, 169, s7–s26. https://doi.org/doi: 10.1086/510141

Heinrich, B. (1975). Bee Flowers: A Hypothesis on Flower Variety and Blooming Times. Evolution,

29(2), 325–334. https://doi.org/doi:10.1111/j.1558-5646.1975.tb00212.x

Hempel de Ibarra, N., Giufa, M., & Vorobyev, M. (2001). Detection of Coloured Patterns by Honeybees

Through Chromatic and Achromatic Cues. Journal of Comparative Physiology A, 187(3), 215–

224. https://doi.org/10.1007/s003590100192

Hernández-Yáñez, H., Lara-Rodríguez, N., Díaz-Castelazo, C., Dáttilo, W., & Rico-Gra, V. (2013).

Understanding the Complex Structure of a Plant-Floral Visitor Network from Different

Perspectives in Coastal Veracruz, Mexico. Sociobiology, 60(3), 329–336. https://doi.org/DOI:

10.13102/sociobiology.v60i3.329-336

Herrera, G., Zagal, J. C., Diaz, A. G., Fernandez, M. J., Vielma, A., Cure, M., … Palacios, A. G. (2008).

Spectral Sensitivities of Photoreceptors and Their Role in Colour Discrimination in the Green-

Backed Firecrown Hummingbird (Sephanoides sephaniodes). Journal of Comparative Physiology

A, 194, 785–794. https://doi.org/DOI 10.1007/s00359-008-0349-8

Johnson, K. A. (2013). Are the Pollination Syndromes in Australian Epacrids (Ericaceae:

Styphelioideae)? A Novel Statistical Method to Identify Key Floral Traits Per Syndrome. Annals

of Botany, 112, 141–149. https://doi.org/doi:10.1093/aob/mct105

Johnson, S. D., & Wester, P. (2017). Stefan Vogel’s Analysis of Floral Syndromes in the South African

Flora: An Appraisal Based on 60 Years of Pollination Studies. Flora, 232, 200–206.

http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.flora.2017.02.005

Kirschfeld, K., & Franceschini, N. (1978). A Photostable Pigment Within the Rhabdomere of Fly

Photoreceptors no. 7. Journal of Comparative Physiology, 125(3), 275–284.

https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00656606

Page 79: Marilia Fernandes Erickson...Agradeço às minhas irmãs Biny e Becky que, embora não fizeram nenhum bolo de cenoura para mim, estavam sempre com o Ifoods na mão. Aos meus pais,

75

Koshitaka, H., Kinoshita, M., Vorobyev, M., & Arikawa, K. (2008). Tetrachromacy in a Butterfly That

Has Eight Varieties of Spectral Receptors. Proceeding of the Royal Society B, 275(1637), 947–

954. https://doi.org/doi: 10.1098/rspb.2007.1614

Kuniyasu, M., Yumoto, T., Nagamitsu, T., Makoto, K., Nagamasu, H., Sakai, S., … Tamiji, I. (1998).

Pollination Biology in a Lowland Dipterocarp Forest in Sarawak, Malaysia. I. Characteristics of

the Plant-Pollinator Community in a Lowland Dipterocarp Forest. American Journal of Botany,

85, 1477–1501. https://doi.org/10.2307/2446404

Lunau, K. (2014). Visual Ecology of Flies With Particular Reference to Colour Vision and Colour

Preferences. Journal of Comparative Physiology A, 200(6), 497–512.

https://doi.org/doi:10.1007/s00359-014-0895-1

Lunau, K., & Maier, E. J. (1995). Innate Colour Preferencesof Flower Visitors. Journal of Comparative

Physiology A, 177(1), 1–19. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00243394

Lunau, K., Papiorek, S., Eltz, T., & Sazima, M. (2011). Avoidance of Achromatic Colours by Bees

Provides a Private Ninche for Hummingbirds. Journal of Experimental Biology, 214, 1607–1612.

https://doi.org/doi: 10.1242/jeb.052688

Lunau, K., & Wacht, S. (1994). Optical Releasers of the Innate Proboscis Extension in the Hoverfly

Eristalis tenax L. (Syrphidae, Diptera). Journal of Comparative Physiology A, 174(5), 575–579.

https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00217378

Maia, R., Gruson, H., Endler, J. A., & White, T. E. (2018). Pavo 2.0: New Tools for the Spectral and

Spatial Analysis of Colour in R. BioRxiv.

McCulloch, K. J., Osorio, D., & Briscoe, A. D. (2016). Sexual Dimorphism in the Compound Eye of

Heliconius erato: A Nymphalid Butterfly with at Least Five Spectral Classes of Photoreceptor.

Journal of Experimental Biology, 219, 2377–2387. https://doi.org/doi:10.1242/jeb.136523

Ollerton, J., Alarcón, R., Waser, N. M., Prince, M. V., Watts, S., Cranmer, L., … Rotenberry, J. (2009).

A Global Test of the Pollination Syndrome Hypothesis. Annals of Botany, 103(9), 1471–1480.

https://doi.org/10.1093/aob/mcp031

Ollerton, J., Rech, A. R., Waser, N. M., & Prince, M. V. (2015). Using the Literature to Test Pollination

Syndromes —Some Methodological Cautions. Journal of Pollination Ecology, 16(17), 119–125.

http://dx.doi.org/10.26786/1920-7603%282015%2917

Olsson, P., Lind, O., & Kelber, A. (2018). Chromatic and Achromatic Vision: Parameter Choice and

Limitations for Reliable Model Predictions. Behavioral Ecology, 9(2), 273–282.

https://doi.org/10.1093/beheco/arx133

Osorio, D., & Vorobyev, M. (1996). Colour Vision as an Adaptation to Frugivory in Primates.

Proceeding of the Royal Society B, 263, 593–599.

Page 80: Marilia Fernandes Erickson...Agradeço às minhas irmãs Biny e Becky que, embora não fizeram nenhum bolo de cenoura para mim, estavam sempre com o Ifoods na mão. Aos meus pais,

76

Peitsch, D., Fietz, A., Hertel, H., Souza, J., Ventura, D. F., & Menzel, R. (1992). The Spectral Input

Systems of Hymenopteran Insects and Their Receptor-Based Colour Vision. Journal of

Comparative Physiology A, 170, 23–40.

Raine, N. E., & Chittka, L. (2007). The Adaptive Significance of Sensory Bias in a Foraging Context:

Floral Colour Preferences in the Bumblebee Bombus terrestris. PLoS ONE, 2(6).

https://doi.org/doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0000556

Reverté, S., Retana, J., Gómez, J. M., & Bosch, J. (2016). Pollinators Show Flower Colour Preferences

but Flowers with Similar Colours do Not Attract Similar Pollinators. Annals of Botany, 118(2),

249–257. https://doi.org/10.1093/aob/mcw103

Rosas-Guerrero, V., Aguilar, R., Marten-Rodriguez, S., Ashworth, L., Lopezaraiza-Mikel, M., Bastida,

J. M., & Quesada, M. (2014). A Quantitative Review of Pollination Syndromes: Do Floral Traits

Predict Effective Pollinators? Ecology Letters, 17, 388–400. https://doi.org/doi: 10.1111/ele.12224

Salcedo, E., Huber, A., Henrich, S., Chadwell, L. V., Chou, W. H., Paulsen, R., & Britt, S. G. (1999).

Blue and Green Absorbing Visual Pigments of Drosophila: Ectopic Expression and Physiological

Characterization of the R8 Photoreceptor Cell-Specific Rh5 and Rh6 Rhodopsins. Journal of

Neuroscience, 19(24), 10716–10726. https://doi.org/DOI:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.19-24-

10716.1999

Schaefer, H. M., Schaefer, V., & Levey, D. J. (2004). How Plant-Animal Interactions Signal New

Insights in Communication. Trends in Ecology and Evolution, 19(11), 577–584.

Schiestl, S. D., & Johnson, F. P. (2013). Pollinator-Mediated Evolution of Floral Signals. Trends in

Ecology & Evolution, 28(5), 307–315. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tree.2013.01.019

Troje, N. (1993). Spectral Categories in the Learning Behaviour of Blowflies. Zeitschrift Für

Naturforschung C, 48(1–2), 96–104. https://doi.org/DOI: https://doi.org/10.1515/znc-1993-1-218

Vorobyev, M., Osorio, D., Bennett, A. T., Marshall, N. J., & Cuthill, I. C. (1998). Tetrachromacy, Oil

Droplets and Bird Plumage Colours. Journal of Comparative Physiology A, 183(5), 621–633.

https://doi.org/doi:10.1007/s003590050286

Waser, N. M. (1986). Flower Constancy: Definition, Cause, and Measurement. The American

Naturalist, 127(5), 593–603. https://doi.org/DOI: 10.1086/284507

Waser, N. M., Chittka, L., Prince, M. V., Williams, N. M., & Ollerton, J. (1996). Generalization in

Pollination Systems, and Why it Matters. Ecology, 77(4), 1043–1060.

Weiss, M. R. (1997). Innate Colour Preferences and Flexible Colour Learning in the Pipevine

Swallowtail. Animal Behavior, 53, 1043–1052.

White, T. E., Dalrymple, R. L., Herberstein, M. E., & Kemp, D. J. (2017). The Perceptual Similarity of

Orb-Spider Prey Lures and Flower Colours. Ecology & Evolution, 31(1), 1–20. https://doi.org/DOI

10.1007/s10682-016-9876-x

Page 81: Marilia Fernandes Erickson...Agradeço às minhas irmãs Biny e Becky que, embora não fizeram nenhum bolo de cenoura para mim, estavam sempre com o Ifoods na mão. Aos meus pais,

77

Wilmer, P. (2011). Pollination and Floral Ecology. Princeton: Pinceton University Press.

Page 82: Marilia Fernandes Erickson...Agradeço às minhas irmãs Biny e Becky que, embora não fizeram nenhum bolo de cenoura para mim, estavam sempre com o Ifoods na mão. Aos meus pais,

78

4.8. Supplementary Material

Table S1. List of species used, locations found, and which backgrounds were used for the visual

modeling.

Family Species Biome Background

Acanthaceae Ruellia asperula (Mart. ex Nees) Lindau Thorn Forest Leaf

Harpochilus paraibanus F.K.S. Monteiro,

J.I.M. Melo & E.M.P. Fernando Thorn Forest

Inflorescenc

e

Amaranthaceae Alternanthera tenella Colla Thorn Forest Leaf

Alternanthera brasiliana L. Thorn Forest Leaf

Anacardiaceae Anacardium occidentale L.

Atlantic

Forrest

(Restinga) Leaf

Annonaceae Guatteria schomburgkiana Mart.

Atlantic

Forrest

(Restinga) Leaf

Apocynaceae

Mandevilla moricandiana (A.DC.)

Woodson

Atlantic

Forrest

(Restinga) Leaf

Hancornia speciosa Gomes

Atlantic

Forrest

(Restinga) Leaf

Asteraceae Blainvillea dichotoma (Murray) Stewart Thorn Forest Leaf

Bignoniaceae Handroanthus impetiginosus Thorn Forest Tree trunk

Fridericia dichotoma (Jacq.) L.G.Lohma Thorn Forest Leaf

Boraginaceae Varronia leucocephala (Moric.) J.S.Mill. Thorn Forest Leaf

Varronia globosa Jacq. Thorn Forest Leaf

Burmanniaceae

Gymnosiphon divaricatus (Benth.) Benth.

& Hook.f.

Atlantic

Forrest

(Restinga) Leaf litter

Burseraceae

Commiphora leptophloeos (Mart.) J.B.

Gillett Thorn Forest Leaf

Cactaceae

Tacinga inamoena (K.Schum.) N.P.Taylor

& Stuppy Thorn Forest Leaf litter

Melocactus violaceus Pfeiff.

Atlantic

Forrest

(Restinga)

Inflorescenc

e

Celastraceae Maytenus erythroxyla (Reissek) Biral

Atlantic

Forrest

(Restinga) Leaf

Page 83: Marilia Fernandes Erickson...Agradeço às minhas irmãs Biny e Becky que, embora não fizeram nenhum bolo de cenoura para mim, estavam sempre com o Ifoods na mão. Aos meus pais,

79

Chrysobalanacea

e

Hirtella racemosa (Willd. ex Roem. &

Schult.) Prance

Atlantic

Forrest

(Restinga) Leaf

Hirtella ciliata Mart. & Zucc.

Atlantic

Forrest

(Restinga) Leaf

Commelinaceae Commelina erecta L. Thorn Forest Leaf

Convolvulaceae Jacquemontia pentanthos (Jacq.) G.Don Thorn Forest Leaf

Ipomoea bahiensis Willd. ex Roem. &

Schult. Thorn Forest Leaf

Cucurbitaceae Cayaponia tayuya (Vell.) Cogn. Thorn Forest Leaf

Ceratosanthes palmata (L.) Urb. Thorn Forest Rock

Cyperaceae Rhynchospora cephalotes Ness

Atlantic

Forrest

(Restinga) Leaf

Erythroxylaceae Erythroxylum pungens O.E.Schulz Thorn Forest Leaf

Erythroxylum rimosum O.E.Schulz

Atlantic

Forrest

(Restinga) Leaf

Euphorbiaceae Croton sp. Thorn Forest Leaf

Croton hirtus L'Hér. Thorn Forest Leaf

Ditaxis desertorum (Müll.Arg.) Pax &

K.Hoffm. Thorn Forest Leaf

Dalechampia sp. Thorn Forest Leaf

Jatropha mollissima (Pohl) Baill. Thorn Forest Leaf

Sebastiania sp. Thorn Forest Leaf

Fabaceae Indigofera sp. Thorn Forest Leaf

Canavalia brasiliensis Mart. ex Benth. Thorn Forest Leaf

Trischidium molle (Benth.) H.E.Ireland Thorn Forest Leaf

Chamaecrista ramosa (Vogel) H.S.Irwin &

Barneby

Atlantic

Forrest

(Restinga) Leaf

Periandra mediterranea (Vell.) Taub.

Atlantic

Forrest

(Restinga) Leaf

Stylosanthes guianensis (Aubl.) Sw.

Atlantic

Forrest

(Restinga) Leaf

Desmodium barbatum (L.) Benth.

Atlantic

Forrest

(Restinga) Leaf

Stylosanthes capitata Vogel

Atlantic

Forrest

(Restinga) Leaf

Page 84: Marilia Fernandes Erickson...Agradeço às minhas irmãs Biny e Becky que, embora não fizeram nenhum bolo de cenoura para mim, estavam sempre com o Ifoods na mão. Aos meus pais,

80

Gentianaceae Voyria sp.

Atlantic

Forrest

(Forrest) Leaf litter

Voyria aphylla (Jacq.) Pers.

Atlantic

Forrest

(Restinga) Leaf litter

Schultesia sp.

Atlantic

Forrest

(Restinga) Leaf litter

Heliconiaceae Heliconia psittacorum L.f.

Atlantic

Forrest

(Forrest) Leaf

Krameriaceae Krameria tomentosa A.St.-Hil.

Atlantic

Forrest

(Restinga) Leaf

Lamiaceae Mesosphaerum suaveolens (L.) Kuntze Thorn Forest Leaf

Eplingiella fruticosa (Salzm. Ex Benth)

Atlantic

Forrest

(Restinga) Leaf

Lauraceae Cassytha filiformis L.

Atlantic

Forrest

(Restinga) Tree trunk

Leguminosae Zornia diphylla (L.) Pers.

Atlantic

Forrest

(Restinga) Leaf litter

Lentibulariaceae Utricularia sp.

Atlantic

Forrest

(Restinga) Sand

Loasaceae Mentzelia aspera L. Thorn Forest Sand

Loranthaceae Struthanthus syringifolius Mart.

Atlantic

Forrest

(Restinga) Leaf

Psittacanthus dichroos (Mart.) Mart.

Atlantic

Forrest

(Restinga) Leaf

Lythraceae Cuphea flava Spreng.

Atlantic

Forrest

(Restinga) Leaf

Malpighiaceae Stigmaphyllon paralias A.Juss.

Atlantic

Forrest

(Restinga) Leaf

Byrsonima crassifolia (L.) Kunth

Atlantic

Forrest

(Restinga) Leaf

Page 85: Marilia Fernandes Erickson...Agradeço às minhas irmãs Biny e Becky que, embora não fizeram nenhum bolo de cenoura para mim, estavam sempre com o Ifoods na mão. Aos meus pais,

81

Byrsonima gardneriana A. Juss

Atlantic

Forrest

(Restinga) Leaf

Malvaceae Waltheria rotundifolia Schrank Thorn Forest Leaf

Herissantia crispa (L.) Brizicky Thorn Forest Leaf

Sida galheirensis Ulbr. Thorn Forest Leaf

Herissantia tiubae (K.Schum.) Brizicky Thorn Forest Leaf

Pavonia cancellata (L.) Cav. Thorn Forest Sand

Corchorus argutus Kunth Thorn Forest Leaf

Melochia sp. Thorn Forest Leaf

Melastomataceae Comolia villosa (Aubl.) Triana

Atlantic

Forrest

(Restinga) Leaf

Miconia albicans (Sw.) Triana

Atlantic

Forrest

(Restinga) Leaf

Nyctaginaceae Guapira sp.

Atlantic

Forrest

(Restinga) Leaf

Ochnaceae Sauvagesia sprengelii A.St.-Hil.

Atlantic

Forrest

(Restinga) Sand

Ouratea hexasperma (A.St.-Hil.) Baill.

Atlantic

Forrest

(Restinga) Leaf

Orchidaceae Epidendrum cinnabarinum Salzm.

Atlantic

Forrest

(Restinga) Leaf

Vanilla bahiana Hoehne

Atlantic

Forrest

(Restinga) Green Petals

Peraceae Chaetocarpus echinocarpus (Baill.) Ducke

Atlantic

Forrest

(Restinga) Leaf

Polygalaceae Polygala longicaulis Kunth

Atlantic

Forrest

(Restinga) Sand

Asemeia violacea (Aubl.) J.F.B.Pastore &

J.R.Abbott

Atlantic

Forrest

(Restinga) Leaf

Polygonaceae Coccoloba ramosissima Wedd.

Atlantic

Forrest

(Restinga) Leaf

Portulacaceae Portulaca oleracea L. Thorn Forest Leaf

Page 86: Marilia Fernandes Erickson...Agradeço às minhas irmãs Biny e Becky que, embora não fizeram nenhum bolo de cenoura para mim, estavam sempre com o Ifoods na mão. Aos meus pais,

82

Rubiaceae

Richardia grandiflora (Cham. & Schltdl.)

Steud. Thorn Forest Leaf

Psychotria bracteocardia (DC.) Müll.Arg.

Atlantic

Forrest

(Forrest) Leaf

Psychotria sp.

Atlantic

Forrest

(Restinga) Leaf

Staelia virgata (Link ex Roem. & Schult.)

K.Schum.

Atlantic

Forrest

(Restinga) Sand

Perama hirsuta Aubl.

Atlantic

Forrest

(Restinga) Sand

Salzmannia nitida DC.

Atlantic

Forrest

(Restinga) Leaf

Cordiera myrciifolia (K.Schum.)

C.H.Perss. & Delprete

Atlantic

Forrest

(Both) Leaf

Sapindaceae Cardiospermum corindum L. Thorn Forest Leaf

Allophylus sp. Thorn Forest Leaf

Talinaceae Talinum triangulare Willd. Thorn Forest Leaf

Turneraceae Turnera subulata Sm. Thorn Forest Leaf

Turnera cearensis Urb. Thorn Forest Leaf

Verbenaceae Lantana camara L. Thorn Forest Leaf

Lantana radula Sw.

Atlantic

Forrest

(Restinga) Leaf

Lippia alba (Mill.) N.E.Br. ex P. Wilson Thorn Forest Leaf

Xyridaceae Xyris sp.

Atlantic

Forrest

(Restinga) Sand

Page 87: Marilia Fernandes Erickson...Agradeço às minhas irmãs Biny e Becky que, embora não fizeram nenhum bolo de cenoura para mim, estavam sempre com o Ifoods na mão. Aos meus pais,

83

Table S2. Description of flower color categories, proposed by Wilmer (2011), and the number of

flowers sampled in our study.

Color category Description

Number of flowers sampled

Without UV With UV Total

Red Pure bright red 0 0 0 Mauve Dark muted red 2 1 3

Pink Very light red 0 1 1

Orange Bright yellow tinged red or bright red tinged

yellow 1 3 4 Yellow Pure bright yellow 6 10 16 Cream Very light yellow 12 1 13

Green Low contrast between green leaves and

flowers 1 1 2 Greenish Yellow with a small tinge of blue 8 2 10

Pale green Low saturated green 1 0 1 Blue Bright or pale blue 4 0 4

Purple A mixture between pink and blue 9 2 11 White Not tinted by other colors 10 3 13

Dull white Off-white, white with a small tinge of any

other color 15 2 17 Dull beige Muted brownish cream 0 0 0

Dull Light and unsaturated any color 0 0 0 Brown Dark, unsaturated orange or red 1 0 1

Mottled Irregular patches 0 0 0

Page 88: Marilia Fernandes Erickson...Agradeço às minhas irmãs Biny e Becky que, embora não fizeram nenhum bolo de cenoura para mim, estavam sempre com o Ifoods na mão. Aos meus pais,

84

Figure S1 Compared reflectance of flowers measured with Ocean Optics RPH-1 probe holder and

our custom-made 3D printed probe holder. a) Petal of Delonix regia b) Petal of Catharanthus roseus

c) Petal of Plumeria pudica.

Figure S2. Illuminant of Caatinga (a) and Restinga (b) used in visual modeling.

Page 89: Marilia Fernandes Erickson...Agradeço às minhas irmãs Biny e Becky que, embora não fizeram nenhum bolo de cenoura para mim, estavam sempre com o Ifoods na mão. Aos meus pais,

85

5. CONCLUSÃO GERAL

Page 90: Marilia Fernandes Erickson...Agradeço às minhas irmãs Biny e Becky que, embora não fizeram nenhum bolo de cenoura para mim, estavam sempre com o Ifoods na mão. Aos meus pais,

86

Conclusão geral

Embora os estudos de coloração floral têm crescido ao longo das décadas, ainda existem

várias lacunas a serem preenchidas. Estudos que levam em consideração a visão de cores de

diferentes animais precisam ser realizados, especialmente animais que podem ser prejudiciais as

flores (herbívoros, florívoros, ladrões de néctar e pólen). Além disso existe uma escassez de

estudos que avaliam padrões biogeográficos, sendo a maioria realizados na Europa. São

necessários mais estudos que utilizam a história evolutiva para quantificar vias bioquímicas que

podem levar a mudança de coloração em flores, seja ao longo da história evolutiva ou ao longo do

tempo de vida de indivíduos. No geral, coloração floral é uma área que ainda tem muito a ser

explorado.

Síndromes de polinização tem sido uma ferramenta importante nos estudos de coloração

floral, porém, falta bases empíricas que expliquem o porquê certas características estão associadas

com certos polinizadores. Quando comparando qual coloração é mais conspícua para diferentes

polinizadores os padrões que encontramos não seguem os descritos por síndromes de polinização.

Além disso, polinizadores não detectam melhor flores de sua própria síndrome quando comparado

com flores de outras síndromes. Quando comparando polinizadores, flores são mais conspícuas

para polinizadores tetracromatas do que para polinizadores tricromatas. flores não são mais

conspícuas para polinizadores previstos por síndromes, e a coloração das flores não determinou

qual animal a detectava melhor. Mais estudos são necessários para entender por que polinizadores

tendem a visitar flores de certas colorações.

Page 91: Marilia Fernandes Erickson...Agradeço às minhas irmãs Biny e Becky que, embora não fizeram nenhum bolo de cenoura para mim, estavam sempre com o Ifoods na mão. Aos meus pais,

87

6.0 APÊNDICES

Page 92: Marilia Fernandes Erickson...Agradeço às minhas irmãs Biny e Becky que, embora não fizeram nenhum bolo de cenoura para mim, estavam sempre com o Ifoods na mão. Aos meus pais,

MINISTÉRIO DA EDUCAÇÃO COMISSÃO DE ÉTICA NO USO DE ANIMAIS – CEUA

Av. Salgado Filho, S/N – CEP: 59072-970 – Natal / RNFone: (84) 9229-6491 / e-mail: [email protected]

CERTIFICADO

Natal (RN), 24 de abril de 2018.

Certificamos que a proposta intitulada “Comparação da coloração floral entre a caatinga e

a mata atlântica”, protocolo 009/2018, CERTIFICADO nº 089.009/2018 , sob a responsabilidade

de Daniel Marques de Almeida Pessoa - que envolve a produção, manutenção e/ou

utilização de animais pertencentes ao filo Chordata, subfilo Vertebrata (exceto o homem), para

fins de pesquisa científica (ou ensino) - encontra-se de acordo com os preceitos da Lei n.º

11.794, de 8 de outubro de 2008, do Decreto n.º 6.899, de 15 de julho de 2009, e com as

normas editadas pelo Conselho Nacional de Controle da Experimentação Animal (CONCEA),

foi aprovada, após adequações, pela COMISSÃO DE ÉTICA NO USO DE ANIMAIS da

Universidade Federal do Rio Grande do Norte – CEUA/UFRN.

Vigência do Projeto Maio 2019

RELATÓRIO JUNHO 2019

Espécie/Linhagem -

Número de Animais

Não haverá coletada ou manipulação de animais durante a pesquisa. Todas as informações que serão utilizadas na modelagem referentes a animais serão retiradas da literatura. Apenas espécies vegetais serão coeltadas.

Idade/Peso -

Sexo -

Coleta das espécies vegetais

Floresta Nacional de Assu – RN e Reserva Biológica Guaribas - PB

Informamos ainda que, segundo o Cap. 2, Art. 13, do Regimento Interno desta CEUA, é

função do professor/pesquisador responsável pelo projeto a elaboração de relatório de

acompanhamento que deverá ser entregue tão logo a pesquisa seja concluída. O

descumprimento desta norma poderá inviabilizar a submissão de projetos futuros.

José de Castro Souza Neto JúniorCoordenador da CEUA-UFRN

www.ceua.propesq.ufrn.br